Skip to main content

Enkinaesthesia: The Essential Sensuous Background for Co-Agency

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: New Directions in Philosophy and Cognitive Science ((NDPCS))

Abstract

The primary aim of this chapter is to present a case for a heavily revised notion of heterophenomenology. 1 will refer to the revised notion as ‘enkinaesthesia’ because of its dependence on the experiential entanglement of our own and the other’s felt action as the sensory background within which all other experience is possible. Enkinaesthesia1 emphasizes two things: (i) the neuromuscular dynamics of the agent, including the givenness and ownership of its experience and (ii) the entwined, blended and situated co-affective feeling of the presence of the other(s), agential (for example, human, horse, cat, beetle) and non-agential (for example, cup, bed, apple, paper) and, where appropriate, the anticipated arc of the other’s action or movement, including, again where appropriate, the other’s intentionality. When the ‘other’ is also a sensing and experiencing agent it is their — in this case, the pair’s — affective intentional reciprocity, their folding, enfolding and unfolding, which co-constitutes the conscious relation and the experientially recursive temporal dynamics that lead to the formation and maintenance of the deep integral enkinaesthetic structures and melodies which bind us together, even when they pull us apart. Such deeply felt enkinaesthetic melodies emphasize the dialogical nature of the backgrounded feeling of being.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Allport, D. A. (1987) ‘Selection for action: Some behavioral and neurophysiological considerations of attention and action’ in H. Heuer and F. Sanders (eds.) Perspectives on Perception and Action (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum).

    Google Scholar 

  • Bräten, S. (2009) The Intersubjective Mirror in Infant Learning and Evolution of Speech (Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Beniamins).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Chamberlain, D. B. (1995) ‘Communication Before Language’, http://www.birthpsy-chology.com/lifebefore/comm.html, accessed 20 September 2010.

  • Chamberlain, D. B. (1997) ‘The Fetal Senses: A Classical View’, http://www.birthpsy-chology.com/lifebeforegetalsense.html, accessed 20 September 2010.

  • Cichetti, D. and Cohen, D. J. (2006) Developmental Psychopathology, Vol. 2, (New York: John Wiley & Sons Inc )

    Google Scholar 

  • ,Clark, A. (1997) Being There: Putting Brain, Body, and World Together Again (Cambridge, MA/London: MIT Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, A. (2008) Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Cotterill, R. M. J. (1995) ‘On the unity of conscious experience’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(4–5), 290–311.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cotterill, R. M. J. (1998) Enchanted Looms: Conscious Networks in Brains and Computer (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (2003) ‘Who’s on first? Heterophenomenology explained’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 10 (9–10), 19–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett, D. C. (2007) ‘Heterophenomenology Reconsidered’, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6 (1–2), 247–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Depraz, N. (2008) ‘The rainbow of emotions: at the crossroads of neurobiology and phenomenology’, Continental Philosophy Review, 41 (2), 237–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Preester, H. (2008) ‘From ego to alter ego: Husserl, Merleau-Ponty and a layered approach to intersubjectivity’, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 7 (1), 133–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1896) ‘The reflex arc concept in psychology’, Psychological Review, 3, 357–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Pellegrino, G., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, V. Gallese, and G. Rizzolatti (1992) ’Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study’, Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. and A. Meltzoff, (1996) ‘The earliest sense of self and others: Merleau-Ponty and recent developmental studies’, Philosophical Psychology, 9 (2), 211–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, and G. Rizzolatti (1996) ‘Action recognition in the premotor cortex’, Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979) The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception (Boston: Houghton Mifflin).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, J. (2009) ‘Embodying meaning: Insights from primates, autism, and Brentano’, Neural Networks, 22(2), 190–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, J. (2010) ‘The emergence of eye contact as an intersubjective signal in an infant gorilla: implications for models of early social cognition’, Acción Psicológica, 7(2), 35–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, J. (2011) ‘The ontogeny of triadic cooperative interactions with humans in an infant gorilla’ Interaction Studies 11(3) 353–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G. (1970) ‘Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideomotor mechanism’. Psychological Review, 77, 73–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1962) Being and Time (London: SCM Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hommel, B., J. Messeler, G. Aschersleben, and W. Prinz (1998) ‘The Theory of Event Coding (TEC): A Framework for Perception and Action’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–937.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hundert, E. M. (1989) Philosophy, Psychiatry and Neuroscience—Three Approaches to the Mind: A Synthetic Analysis of the Varieties of Human Experience (Oxford: Clarendon Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Hurley, S. L. and A Noë (2003) ‘Neural Plasticity and Consciousness’, Biology and Philosophy, 18, 131–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1959) Erste Philosophie (1923/4) Erster Teil: Kritische Ideengeschichte (First Philosophy (1923/24), Part 1: Critical History of Ideas), R. Boehm (ed.) (The Hague, NerhPrlands: Martinus Nijhoff).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1964) On the Phenomenology of the Consciousness of Internal Time (Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1970) The Crisis of European Sciences and Transcendental Philosophy, (1936/54). D. Carr. trans. (Evanston: Northwestern University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1973) Zur Phänomenologie der Intersubjektivität, Dreiter Teil: 1929–1935, Husserliana (Vol. 15) (The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1977) Phenomenological Psychology, trans. J. Scanlon (The Hague: Martinus Nijhoff).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1983) Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Pheno-menological Philosophy, First Book (Ideas I), trans. F. Kersten (The Hague: Martinus Niihoff).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1989) Ideas Pertaining to a Pure Phenomenology and to a Phenomenological Philosophy, Second Book (Ideas II), trans. R. Rojcewicz and A. Schuwer (Dordrecht: Kluwer).

    Google Scholar 

  • Husserl, E. (1991) Cartesian Meditations: An Introduction to Phenomenology, D. Cairns (trans.) (The Hague: Nijhoff, Kluwer Academic Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890/1981) The Principles of Psychology (Macmillan/Harvard University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (1994) ‘The representing brain Neural correlates of motor intention and imagery’ Behavioral and Brain Science, 17, 87–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lecanuet, J.-P., W. P. Fifer, N. A. Krasnegor and W. P. Smotherman (eds.) (1995) Fetal Development: A Psychobiological Perspective (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum).

    Google Scholar 

  • Lotze, R. H. (1852) Medicinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele (Leipzig: Weidmann/ 1966 Amsterdam: Bonset).

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1973) The Working Brain: An Introduction to Neuropsychology, transl. B. Haigh (London: Allen Lane).

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch, S. and C. Trevarthen (2009) (eds.) Communicative Musicality: Exploring the Basis of Human Companionship (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Marbach, E. (2007) “No heterophenomenology without autophenomenology: Variations on a theme of mine”, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6 (1–2), 75–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markos, A., F. Grygar, L. Hajnal, K. Kleisner, Z. Kratochvil, and Z. Neubauer (2009) Life as Its Own Designer: Darwin’s Origins and Western Thought (Biosemiotics) (Dordrecht: Springer).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H. (1988) ‘Ontology of observing: The biological foundations of self-consciousness and the physical domain of existence’, Texts in Cybernetic Theory, American Society for Cybernetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962) Phenomenology of Perception, C. Smith (trans.) (London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1963) The Structures of Behavior, A. L. Fisher (trans.) (Boston: Beacon Press)

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Regan, J.K. and A. Noë (2001) ‘A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness’. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24 (5), 939–1031.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998a) ‘The periconscious substrates of consciousness: Affective states and the evolutionary origins of the self’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, 5 (5–6), 566–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Panksepp, J. (1998b) Affective Neuroscience: The Foundations of Human and Animal Emotions (New York: Oxford University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piontelli, A. (1992) From Fetus to Child: An Observational and Psychoanalytic Study (London/New York: Tavistock/Routledge).

    Google Scholar 

  • Piontelli, A. (2002) Twins: From Fetus to Child (London: Routledge).

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., M. H. Harries, R. Bevan, S. Thomas, P. J. Benson, A. J. Mistlin, A. J. Chitty, J. K. Hietanen, and J. E. Ortega (1989) ‘Frameworks of analysis for the neural representation of animate objects and actions’, Journal of Experimental Biology, 146, 87–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perrett, D. I., A. J. Mistlin, H. M. Harries, and A. J. Chitty (1990) ‘Understanding the visual appearance and consequence of hand actions’ in M. A. Goodale (ed.) Vision and Action: The Control of Grasping (Norwood, NJ: Ablex), 163–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe, M. (2008) Feelings of Being: Phenomenology, Psychiatry and the Sense of Reality (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2008) How Infants Know Minds (Cambridge MA: Harvard University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ricœur, P. (1981) Hermeneutics and the Human Sciences (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., L. Fadiga, L. Fogassi, and V. Gallese (1996) ‘Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions’, Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., L. Fogassi and V. Gallese (2001) ‘Neurophysioiogical mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action’, Nature Reviews, Neuroscience, 2, 661–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti G., and L. Craighero (2004) ‘The mirror-neuron system’, Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scerif, G., J. C. Gomez, and R. W. Byrne (2004) ‘What do Diana monkeys know about the focus of attention of a conspecific?’ Animal Behaviour, 68, 1239–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1990) The Roots of Thinking (Temple University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (1999) The Primacy of Movement (Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2000) ‘Kinetic tactile-kinesthetic bodies: Ontogenetical foundations of apprenticeship learning’, Human Studies, 23, 343–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2003) ‘Kinesthetic Memory’, Theoria et Historia Scientiarum, 7, 69–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2009) ‘Animation: The fundamental, essential, and properly descriptive concept’, Continental Philosophy Review 42(3), 375–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, W. (1994) ‘The organization of sensory motor representations in the neo-cortex: A hypothesis based on temporal coding’ in C. Umiltà and M. Moscovitch (eds.) Conscious and nonconscious information processing: Attention and performance XV (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (2000) The Interpersonal World of the Infant: A View from Psychoanalysis and Development Psychology, (Second Edition, with new Introduction) (New York: Basic Books).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (2010) Forms of Vitality: Exploring Dynamic Experience in Psychology and the Arts (Oxford: Oxford University Press).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sherrington, S. S. (1947/1906) The Physiological Position and Dominance of the Brain (New Haven, CT: Yale University Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, S. A. J. (2006) ‘Extended Body, Extended Mind: The Self as Prosthesis’ in R. Pepperell and M. Punt (eds.) Screen Consciousness: Mind, Cinema and World (Amsterdam, New York: Rodopi).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, S. A. J. (2007) ‘Machine Consciousness: Cognitive and Kinaesthetic Imagination’, Journal of Consciousness Studies, Imprint Academic, 14 (7), 141–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, S. A. J. (2008) ‘From Agency to Apperception: Through Kinaesthesia to Cognition and Creation’, Journal of Ethics and Information Technology, 10, 255–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, S. A. J. (2010a) ‘Conscious Machines: Memory, Melody and Imagination’, Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(1) 37–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, S. A. J. (2010b) ‘Enkinaesthetia, Biosemiotics and the Ethiosphere’ in Cowley, S. J., J. C. Major S. Steffensen and A. Dinis (eds.) Signifying Bodies: Biosemiosis, Interaction and Health, (Braga: Portuguese Catholic University Press), 305–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. G. (2010) ‘Mind-body problem: New approaches’, http://www.scholarpedia.org/article/Mind-body_problem

  • Trevarthen, C., K. J. Aitken, M. Vandekerckhove, J. Delafieldt-Butt and E. Nagy (2006) ‘Collaborative Regulations of Vitality in Early Childhood: Stress in Intimate Relationships and Post natal Psychopathology’ in Cichetti and Cohen (2006).

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. and V. Reddy (2007) ‘Consciousness in infants’ in M. Velman and S. Schneider (Eds.) A Companion to Consciousness (Oxford: Blackwells), 41–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. (1996) ‘Neurophenomenology: A methodological remedy for the hard problem’ Journal of Consciousness Studies, 3, 330–49

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. (1999) ‘Steps to a science of Interbeing: Unfolding the Dharma implicit in modern cognitive science’ in G. Watson, S. Batchelor, and G. Claxton (eds.) The Psychology of Awakening: Buddhism, Science and Our Day-to-day Lives (New York, NY: Rider/Random House), 71–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., E. Thompson, and E. Rosch (2003). The Embodied Mind: Cognitive Science and Human Extierience (Cambridge: MIT Press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Velman, M. and S. Schneider (eds.) (2007) A Companion to Consciousness (Oxford: Blackwells).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2002) ‘Six Views of Embodied Cognition’, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9 (4), 625–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953) Philosophical Investigations, G. E. M. Anscombe (trans.) (New York: Macmillan).

    Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, D. (1994) ‘Husserl’s Phenomenology of the Body’, Études Phénoménologiques, 19, 63–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeki, S. (1991) ‘Cerebral akinetopsia (visual motion blindness): A review’, Brain, 114, 811–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zihl, J., D. von Cramon, and N. Mai (1983) ‘Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage’, Brain, 106, 313–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, F., F. Zemke, J. Call, J. Gomez, and M. Tomasello (2009) ‘Orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) point to inform a human about the location of a tool’, Animal Cognition 12, 347–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2012 Susan A. J. Stuart

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Stuart, S.A.J. (2012). Enkinaesthesia: The Essential Sensuous Background for Co-Agency. In: Radman, Z. (eds) Knowing without Thinking. New Directions in Philosophy and Cognitive Science. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230368064_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics