Skip to main content

Contributions of Mirror Mechanisms to the Embodiment of Cognition

  • Chapter
  • 223 Accesses

Part of the book series: New Directions in Philosophy and Cognitive Science ((NDPCS))

Abstract

Mirror neurons in the macaque are equally active when the animal initiates an action and when the animal observes another take the same or similar action. These neurons may be used in goal recognition (I recognize the goal of your action through resonance of that part of my motor system used by me to accomplish a particular goal), inferring intent, empathy, and, in general, greasing the wheels of social interaction. I describe in this chapter a new methodology for investigating mirror mechanism function: because some mirror neurons are part of the motor system they can be adapted through repeated action; and, because mirror neurons are multimodal, the effect of that adaptation can be shown in other cognitive tasks, thereby demonstrating the causal effect of the mirror mechanism on that other cognitive task.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aziz-Zadeh, L., Wilson, S. M., Rizzolatti, G., and Iacoboni, M. (2006). Congruent embodied representations for visually presented actions and linguistic phrases describing actions. Current Biology, 16, 1,818–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Grezes, J., Glaser, D. E., Passingham, D., and Haggard, P. (2006). Seeing or doing? Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Current Biology, 16, 1,905–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cattaneo, L., Barchiesi, G., Tabarelli, D., Arfeller, C., Sato, M., and Glenberg, A. M. (2011). One’s motor performance predictably modulates the understanding of others’actions through adaptation of premotor visuo-motor neurons. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6, 301–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chong, T. T.-J., Cunnington, R., Williams, M. A., Kanwisher, N., and Mattingley, J. B. (2008). FMRI adaptation reveals mirror neurons in human inferior parietal cortex. Current Biology, 18, 1,576–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Classen, J., Liepert, J., Wise, S. P., Hallett, M., and Cohen, L. G. (1998). Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. Journal of Neurophysiology, 79, 1,117–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., and Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 28–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • D’Ausilio, A., Pulvermuller, F., Salmas, P., Buflari, I., Begliomini, C., and Fadiga, L. (2009). The motor somatotopy of speech perception. Current Biology, 19, 381–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Behrmann, M., and Heeger, D. J. (2008). A mirror up to nature. Current Biology, 18, R13–R18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fazio, P., Cantagallo, A., Craighero, L., D’Ausilio, A., Roy, A. C., Pozzo, T., Calzolari, F., Granieri, E., and Fadiga, L. (2009). Encoding of human action in Broca’s area. Brain, doi:10.1093/brain/awp118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galantucci, B., Fowler, C. A., and Turvey, M. T. (2006). The motor theory of speech perception reviewed. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 13, 361–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., and Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, and Rizzolatti, G. (1996). Action recognition in the premotor cortex. Brain, 119, 593–609.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V., Keysers, C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2004). A unifying view of the basis of social cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 396–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gazzola, V., Aziz-Zadeh, L., and Keysers, C. (2006). Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans. Current Biology, 16, 1,824–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (2006). Naturalizing Cognition: The Integration of Cognitive Science and Biology. Current Biology, 16, R802–R804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (2010). Embodiment as a unifying perspective for psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 586–96.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., and Kaschak, M. P. (2002). Grounding language in action. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 558–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., and Robertson, D. A. (1999). Indexical understanding of instructions. Discourse Processes, 28, 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Robertson, D. A., Jansen, J. L., and Johnson-Glenberg, M. C. (1999). Not propositions. Journal of Cognitive Systems Research, 1, 19–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., and Cattaneo, L. (2008a). Use-induced motor plasticity affects the processing of abstract and concrete language. Current Biology, 18, R290–R291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., Sato, M., Cattaneo, L., Riggio, L., Palumbo, D., and Buccino, G. (2008b). Processing abstract language modulates motor system activity. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 905–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Havas, D. A., Glenberg, A. M., and Rinck, M. (2007). Emotion simulation during language comprehension. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 14, 436–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Havas, D. A., Glenberg, A. M., Gutowski, K., Lucarelli, M, and Davidson, R. (2010). Cosmetic use of botulinum toxin-A affects processing of emotional language. Psychological Science, 21, 895–900.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hickock, G. (2009). Eight problems for the mirror neuron theory of action understanding in monkeys and humans. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 1,229–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hickock, G., and Hauser, M. (2010). (Mis)understanding mirror neurons. Current Biology, 20, 593–4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J. M., Marchant, J. L., and Frith, C. D. (2006). Modulation of the mirror system by social relevance. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 1, 143–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J. M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., and Frith, C. D. (2009). Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 10,153–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kohler, E., Keysers, C., Umiltà, M. A., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2002). Hearing Sounds, Understanding Actions: Action Representation in Mirror Neurons. Science, 297, 846–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, C. F., and McBeath, M. K. (2004). Bias to experience approaching motion in a three-dimensional virtual environment. Perception, 33, 259–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M. (1957). Some results of research on speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 29, 117–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lingnau, A., Gesierich, B., and Caramazza, A. (2009). Asymmetric fMRI Adaptation Reveals No Evidence for Mirror Neurons in Humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 106, 9925–9930.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mahon, B. Z. (2008). Action recognition: Is it a motor process? Current Biology, 18, R1,068–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mukamel, R., Ekstrom, A. D., Kaplan, K., Iacoboni, M., and Fried, I. (2010). Single-neuron responses in humans during execution and observation of actions. Current Biology, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.02.045.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pazzaglia, M., Pizzamiglio, L., Pes, E., and Aglioti, S. M. (2008). The sound of actions in apraxia. Current Biology, 18, 1,766–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramachandran, V. S. (2005). MIRROR NEURONS and imitation learning as the driving force behind “the great leap forward” in human evolution. Nova, Science Now website.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., and Arbib. M. A. (1998). Language within our grasp. Trends in Neuorscience, 21, 188–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., and Craighero, L. (2007). Language and mirror neurons. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, pp. 771–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rueschemeyer, S., Glenberg, A. M., Kaschak, M., Mueller, K., and Friederici, A. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up contributions to understanding sentences describing objects in motion. Front. Psychology doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00183.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sato, M., Brisebois, A., Grabski, K., Basirat, A, Ménard, L, Glenberg, A. M., and Cattaneo, L. (2011). Articulatory bias in speech categorization: Evidence from use-induced motor plasticity. Cortex, 47, 1001–1003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saygin, A. P., McCollough, S., Alac, M., and Emmorey, K. (2010). Modulation of BOLD Response in Motion-sensitive Lateral Temporal Cortex by Real and Fictive Motion Sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2,849–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tettamanti, M., Buccino, G., Saccuman, M. C., Gallese, V., Danna, M., Scifo, P., Fazio, F., Rizzolatti, G., Cappa, S. F., and Perani, D. (2005). Listening to actionrelated sentences activates fronto-parietal motor circuits. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, no. 2, 273–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tyrrell R. A., and Owens, D. A. (1988). A rapid technique to assess the resting states of the eyes and other threshold phenomena – The modified binary search (MOBS). Behavior Research Methods Instruments and Computers, 20, 137–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Umilta, M. A., Kohler, E., Gallese, V., Fogassi, L., Fadiga, L., Keysers, C., and Rizzolatti, G. (2001). I know what you are doing: A neurophysiological study. Neuron, 31, 155–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Umilta, M. A., Escola, L., Intskirvell, I., Grammont, F., Rochat, M., Caruana, F., Jezzini, A., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2008). When pliers become fingers in the monkey motor system. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, 105, 2,209–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zwaan, R. A., and Taylor, L. J. (2006). Seeing, acting, understanding: motor resonance in language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 135, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2012 Arthur M. Glenberg

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Glenberg, A.M. (2012). Contributions of Mirror Mechanisms to the Embodiment of Cognition. In: Schulkin, J. (eds) Action, Perception and the Brain. New Directions in Philosophy and Cognitive Science. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/9780230360792_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics