Skip to main content

The Curriculum of Embodied Perception

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Reconstructing 'Education' through Mindful Attention

Abstract

This chapter explores the inner curriculum from the perspective of the brain-mind-body and its process of moment-to-moment embodied perception, most of which occurs beneath our awareness. In every moment this brain-mind-body faces the ultimate curricular question: ‘what ought I attend out of all that is possible?’ Based on cognitive theory, this chapter examines what is my brain-mind-body’s curricular agenda, and how are we ‘educated’ by it? This entails both an exploration of the selective nature of attention, and an analysis of the process of perception. Perception is a recursive process. It is based on an embodied mind, which is grounded in a past and defined by a present state of being. Both affect what and how we attend and yield this moment’s teaching.

The brain is not just a tool for grappling with the world. It’s what brings the world about. McGilchrist 2009, p. 19

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 34.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 44.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    See also McGilchrist: “[T]hings change according to the stance we adopt towards them, the type of attention we give to them, the disposition we hold in relation to them” (2009, p. 4).

  2. 2.

    This is what Kahneman (2011) describes as our automatic and primary ‘system 1’ that ‘thinks fast’.

  3. 3.

    This is a very fundamental basis of perception to which neuroscientists refer as the dual-processing that combines bottom-up sensual data, with top-down representations (Hochstein and Ahissar 2002). Basically, to make our perception far more efficient and quicker, once we’ve learned to read or to identify an object, our brains constantly anticipate what’s coming next. One can think of it as a scientist-in-the-mind that makes a conjecture and changes it only if it doesn’t work. The brain is thus an “anticipation machine” (Siegel 2012, p. 128).

  4. 4.

    “There is general agreement that emotion states bear two important phenomenal features, one mental and the other bodily…we will refer to these two components of emotion as valence (a subjective feeling of pleasantness or unpleasantness) and arousal (extent of bodily excitation), respectively” (Berkovich-Ohana and Glikshon 2014, pp. 11–12).

  5. 5.

    The term state of being parallels the term ‘mental state’ as applied in Siegel (2012).

  6. 6.

    See also Ellsworth and Scherer (2003) and Davidson et al. (2003).

  7. 7.

    There’s no coincidence in the induction of fear as a way to get students’ attention. It certainly gets students’ attention for that moment though unfortunately, evidence shows that stress has a damaging effect on memory encoding (Elzinga et al. 2005).

  8. 8.

    This is especially the case in conditions such as post-traumatic stress disorder (Davidson 2012; Elzinga et al. 2005)

  9. 9.

    What Siegel refers to as “the first phase of emotional response” seems to be what some have analyzed as the “feeling tone” when discussing cognition from a Buddhist perspective (Grabovac et al. 2011; Varela et al. 1991, p. 67).

  10. 10.

    By the way, running is exactly the wrong thing to do in such case…

  11. 11.

    I use this “shout”-pedagogy when I teach philosophy of education, curriculum theory, social-emotional learning, and other courses.

  12. 12.

    See Nummenmaa et al. (2014) for an illuminating research in the domain of emotional body maps.

  13. 13.

    There is growing evidence from neuroscience that associates this self-other relationship with the functioning of the insula; especially in the research of compassion (Singer et al. 2009; Zaki et al. 2012).

  14. 14.

    There are some interesting parallels here with Kahneman’s (2011) ‘system 1’ vs. ‘system 2’ though I believe the theory proposed here accredits the mind with more potential.

  15. 15.

    “arousal leads to enhanced encoding via increased neuronal plasticity and the creation of new synaptic connections and therefore increased likelihood of future retrieval” (Siegel 2012, p. 163). See also Phelps and Sharot (2008).

  16. 16.

    “emotionally arousing experiences become better remembered by a combination of direct physiological effects (perhaps on the genetic activation leading to synapses formation…) and complex cognitive effects on the encoding of memory via the retrieval, rehearsal, and reencoding process” (Siegel 2012, p. 76).

  17. 17.

    Kahneman’s Thinking Fast and Slow (2011) is a book that applies various cognitive experiments, which effectively bring embodied perception into the reader’s experience. It constitutes a ‘colder’ intellectual approach, nevertheless it can significantly contribute to any development of this domain of the inner curriculum.

References

  • Berkovich-Ohana, A., & Glicksohn, J. (2014). The consciousness state space (CSS)—A unifying model for consciousness and self. Frontiers in Psychology, 5(341), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1991). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience. New York: HarperPerennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Hill, H. (Eds.). (2003). Handbook of affective sciences (Vol. xvii). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., with Begley, S. (2012). The emotional life of your brain. New York: Plume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durlak, J. A., Domitovich, C. E., Weissberg, R. P., & Gullotta, T. P. (2015). Handbook of social and emotional learning: Research and practice. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elzinga, B. M., Bakker, A., & Bremner, J. D. (2005). Stress-induced cortisol elevations are associated with impaired delayed, but not immediate recall. Psychiatry Research, 134(3), 211–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ergas, O. (2013). Descartes in a headstand: Introducing body-oriented pedagogy. Philosophical Inquiry in Education, 21(1), 4–12.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ergas, O. (2014). Overcoming the philosophy/life, body/mind rift. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 46(1), 74–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ergas, O. (2016a). Educating the wandering mind: Pedagogical mechanisms of mindfulness practice for a curricular blind spot. Journal of Transformative Education, 14(2), 98–119.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grabovac, A. D., Lau, M. A., & Willett, B. R. (2011). Mechanisms of mindfulness: A Buddhist psychological model. Mindfulness, 2(3), 154–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanson, R., & Mendius, R. (2009). Buddha’s brain: The practical neuroscience of happiness, love & wisdom. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neuropsychological theory. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hochstein, S., & Ahissar, M. (2002). View from the top: Hierarchies and reverse hierarchies in the visual system. Neuron, 36(5), 791–804.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1984). Psychology, briefer course. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (2007). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Cosimo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus & Giroux.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, P. D. (1990). The triune brain in evolution: Role in paleocerebral functions. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGilchrist, I. (2009). The master and his emissary: The divided brain and the making of the Western world. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menon, V. (2015). Salience network. In A. W. Toga (Ed.), Brain mapping: An encyclopedic reference (Vol. 2, pp. 597–611). Los Angeles, CA: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Nørretranders, T. (1998). The user illusion: Cutting consciousness down to size. New York: Viking Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nummenmaa, L., Glerean, E., Hari, R., & Hietanen, J. K. (2014). Bodily maps of emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(2), 646–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, P. (1983). To know as we are known: A spirituality of education. San Francisco, CA: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phelps, E. A., & Sharot, T. (2008). How (and why) emotion enhances the subjective sense of recollection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(2), 147–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schonert-Reichl, K. A., & Roeser, R. W. (Eds.). (2016). Handbook of mindfulness in education: Integrating theory and research into practice. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatz, C. (1992). The developing brain. Scientific American, 267(3), 60–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, D. (2012). The developing mind: How relationships and the brain interact to shape who we are (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, T., Critchley, H. D., & Preuschoff, K. (2009). A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 334–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, N., & Cowan, N. (1995). The cocktail party phenomenon revisited: How frequent are attention shifts to one’s name in an irrelevant auditory channel? Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 255–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaki, J., Davis, J. I., & Ochsner, K. N. (2012). Overlapping activity in anterior insula during interception and emotional experience. NeuroImage, 62(1), 493–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Copyright information

© 2017 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ergas, O. (2017). The Curriculum of Embodied Perception . In: Reconstructing 'Education' through Mindful Attention. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-58782-4_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-58782-4_7

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, London

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-137-58781-7

  • Online ISBN: 978-1-137-58782-4

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics