Skip to main content

Culture and Psychometric Studies of Creativity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
The Palgrave Handbook of Creativity and Culture Research

Part of the book series: Palgrave Studies in Creativity and Culture ((PASCC))

Abstract

For at least five decades, the field of creativity research was strongly rooted in psychometrics as well as extremely Westernized. Currently, both of these characteristics of creativity literature are vigorously criticized, but both are sources of important findings and continuously influential theoretical concepts. This chapter discusses the main consequences of the psychometric approach to creativity and culture. First, it addresses the question of whether and to what extent the most dominant and popular form of creativity measurements—that is, divergent thinking tests, insight problems, or personality questionnaires and inventories—are cross-culturally valid. It argues that implicit and especially explicit understandings of what creativity is strongly depend on culture and that cross-culturally valid creativity measurement is hardly possible. As the understanding of creativity differs across the world, cross-country differences in fluency, flexibility, or originality tell us little about differences in creativity. Therefore, this chapter calls for a more interactional and dynamic, but at the very same time local and emic measurement of creativity, and provides examples of such measurement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 229.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 299.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Amabile, T. M. (1996). Creativity in context. Boulder: Westview Press, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

  • Barbot, B., Besançon, M., & Lubart, T. I. (2011). Assessing creativity in the classroom. The Open Education Journal, 4, 58–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beghetto, R. A. (2010). Creativity in the classroom. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 447–463). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Benedek, M., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). Revisiting Mednick’s model on creativity-related differences in associative hierarchies. Evidence for a common path to uncommon thought. Journal of Creative Behavior, 47, 273–289.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Benedek, M., Jauk, E., Fink, A., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., et al. (2014). To create or to recall? Neural mechanisms underlying the generation of creative new ideas. NeuroImage, 88, 125–133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, C., Kasof, J., Himsel, A., Dmitrieva, J., Dong, Q., & Xue, G. (2005). Effects of explicit instruction to “be creative” across domains and cultures. Journal of Creative Behavior, 39, 89–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, G. W., & Rensvold, R. B. (2002). Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Structural Equation Modeling, 9, 233–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronbach, L. J., & Shavelson, R. J. (2004). My current thoughts on coefficient alpha and successor procedures. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 64, 391–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cropley, A. J. (2000). Defining and measuring creativity: Are creativity tests worth using? Roeper Review, 23, 72–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1996). Creativity: Flow and the psychology of discovery and invention. New York: Harper/Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Implications of a systems perspective for the study of creativity. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 313–335). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diedrich, J., Benedek, M., Jauk, E., & Neubauer, A. C. (2015). Are creative ideas novel and useful? Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 9, 35–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1999). Self-theories: Their role in motivation, personality and development. Philadelphia: Taylor and Francis/Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (2006). Mindset. New York: Random House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995a). Implicit theories and their role in judgements and reactions: A world from two perspectives. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 267–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S., Chiu, C., & Hong, Y. (1995b). Implicit theories: Elaboration and extension of the model. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 322–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dziedziewicz, D., & Karwowski, M. (2015). Development of children’s creative visual imagination: A theoretical model and enhancement programmes. Education, 3–13(43), 382–392.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1993). Creativity and personality: Suggestions for a theory. Psychological Inquiry, 4, 147–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Genius: The natural history of creativity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Feist, G. J. (1998). A meta-analysis of the impact of personality on scientific and artistic creativity. Personality and Social Psychological Review, 2, 290–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R. A., Ward, T. B., & Smith, S. M. (1992). Creative cognition: Theory, research, and applications. Bradford: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gajda, A., Karwowski, M., & Beghetto, R. A. (2016). Creativity and academic achievement: A meta-analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, Aug 18 , 2016, http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/edu0000133.

  • Glăveanu, V. P. (2010). Paradigms in the study of creativity: Introducing the perspective of cultural psychology. New Ideas in Psychology, 28, 79–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glăveanu, V. P. (2011a). Creativity as cultural participation. Journal for the Theory of Social Behaviour, 41, 48–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glăveanu, V. P. (2011b). Is the lightbulb still on? Social representations of creativity in a western context. International Journal of Creativity and Problem Solving, 21, 53–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glăveanu, V. P. (2012). What can be done with an egg? Creativity, material objects, and the theory of affordances. Journal of Creative Behavior, 46, 192–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glăveanu, V. P. (2014). The psychology of creativity: A critical reading. Creativity: Theories-Research-Applications, 1, 10–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glăveanu, V. P. (in press). Creativity and culture: Four (mis)understandings. In T. Lubart, M. Botella, X. Caroff, C. Mouchiroud, J. Nelson, & F. Zenasni (Eds.), Homo creativus: the 7 C’s of human creativity. Singapore: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glăveanu, V. P., Karwowski, M., Jankowska, D. M., & Saint-Laurent, C. (in press). Creative imagination. In V. P. Glăveanu & T. Zittoun (Eds.), Oxford handbook of culture and imagination. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gralewski, J., & Karwowski, M. (2012). Creativity and school grades: A case from Poland. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 7, 198–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gralewski, J., & Karwowski, M. (2016). Are teachers’ implicit theories of creativity related to the recognition of their students’ creativity? Journal of Creative Behavior, Early View online: 29 JAN 2016, doi: 10.1002/jocb.140

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, H. E., & Barrett, P. H. (1974). Darwin on man: A psychological study of scientific creativity. New York: EP Dutton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1950). Creativity. American Psychologist, 5, 444–454.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gundry, L. K., Kickul, J. R., & Prather, C. W. (1994). Building the creative organization. Organizational Dynamics, 22, 22–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of individualism–collectivism in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 3–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hass, R. W., & Burke, S. (2016). Implicit theories of creativity are differentially categorized by perspective and exemplar domain. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 19, 219–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hass, R. W., Reiter-Palmon, R., & Katz- Buonincontro, J. (in press). Domain-specificity, implicit theories, and creative mindsets. In M. Karwowski & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Creative self. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard. Psychological Review, 106, 766–794.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heine, S. J., Kitayama, S., Lehman, D. R., Takata, T., Ide, E., Leung, C., et al. (2001). Divergent consequences of success and failure in Japan and North America: An investigation of self-improving motivations and malleable selves. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 599–615.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hocevar, D. (1981). Measurement of creativity: Review and critique. Journal of Personality Assessment, 45, 450–460.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Houtz, J. C., & Krug, D. (1995). Assessment of creativity: Resolving a mid-life crisis. Educational Psychology Review, 7, 269–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C., & Gong, Y. (1990). Personality differences between writers and mathematicians on the EPQ. Personality and Individual Differences, 11, 637–638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and postmodernization. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jankowska, D. M., & Karwowski, M. (2015). Measuring creative imagery abilities. Frontiers in Psychology, 6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01591.

  • Jauk, E., Benedek, M., Dunst, B., & Neubauer, A. C. (2013). The relationship between intelligence and creativity: New support for the threshold hypothesis by means of empirical breakpoint detection. Intelligence, 41, 212–221.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jeffrey, B., & Woods, P. (2003). The creative school: A framework for success, quality and effectiveness. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Jellen, H. G., & Urban, K. K. (1989). Assessing creative potential world-wide: The first cross-cultural application of the test of creative thinking-drawing production (TCT-DP). Gifted Education International, 6, 78–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, A. R. (1998). The g factor: The science of mental ability. Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kālis, E., Roķe, L., & Krūmiņa, I. (2016). Investigation of psychometric properties of the test for creative thinking – Drawing production: Evidence from study in Latvia. Journal of Creative Behavior, advanced online publication. doi:10.1002/jocb.68.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M. (2014). Creative mindsets: Measurement, correlates, consequences. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 62–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M. (2015). Notes on creative potential and its measurement. Creativity: Theories – Research – Applications, 2, 4–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M., & Barbot, B. (2016). Creative self-beliefs: Their nature, development, and correlates. In J. C. Kaufman & J. Baer (Eds.), Cambridge companion to reason and development (pp. 302–326). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M., & Gralewski, J. (2013). Threshold hypothesis: Fact or artifact? Thinking Skills and Creativity, 8, 25–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M., & Lebuda, I. (2013). Extending climato-economic theory: When, how, and why it explains differences in nations’ creativity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 493–494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M., Werner, C. H., & Tang, M. (2016). Creative mindset measurement invariance across countries. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M., Dul, J., Gralewski, J., Jauk, E., Jankowska, D. M., Gajda, A., & Benedek, M. (2016). Is creativity without intelligence possible? A Necessary Condition Analysis. Intelligence, 57, 105–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karwowski, M., Jankowska, D., Gajda, A., Marczak, M., Groyecka, A., & Sorokowski, P. (2016). Greater male variability in creativity outside the WEIRD world. Creativity Research Journal, 4, XX–XX.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kashima, Y., Kokubo, T., Kashima, E. S., Boxall, D., Yamaguchi, S., & Macrae, K. (2004). Culture and self: Are there within-culture differences in self between metropolitan areas and regional cities? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 816–823.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kasof, J. (1995). Explaining creativity: The attributional perspective. Creativity Research Journal, 8, 311–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S. (2009). IQ testing 101. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J. C. (2015). Why creativity isn’t in IQ tests, why it matters, and why it won’t change anytime soon probably. Journal of Intelligence, 3, 59–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J. C., & Beghetto, R. A. (2009). Beyond big and little: The four c model of creativity. Review of General Psychology, 13, 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.) (2006). The international handbook of creativity. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J. C., & Sternberg, R. J. (Eds.) (2010). The Cambridge handbook of creativity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khaleefa, O. H., Erdos, G., & Ashira, I. H. (1996). Creativity in an indigenous Afro-Arab Islamic culture: The case of Sudan. Journal of Creative Behavior, 30, 268–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kharkhurin, A. V. (2014). Creativity. 4in1: Four-criterion construct of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 338–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kharkhurin, A. V., & Samadpour Motalleebi, S. N. (2008). The impact of culture on the creative potential of American, Russian, and Iranian college students. Creativity Research Journal, 20, 404–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K. H. (2007). Exploring the interactions between Asian culture (Confucianism) and creativity. Journal of Creative Behavior, 41, 28–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, K. H., Cramond, B., & Bandalos, D. L. (2006). The latent structure and measurement invariance of scores on the Torrance tests of creative thinking–figural. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 66, 459–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirton, M. J. (1976). Adaptors and innovators. A description and measure. Journal of Applied Psychology, 61, 622–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobal, D., & Musek, J. (2001). Self-concept and academic achievement: Slovenia and France. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 887–899.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kozbelt, A., Beghetto, R. A., & Runco, M. A. (2010). Theories of creativity. In J. C. Kaufman & R. J. Sternberg (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of creativity (pp. 20–47). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Krampen, G. (2012). Cross-sequential results on creativity development in childhood within two different school systems: Divergent performances in Luxembourg versus German kindergarten and elementary school students. Europe’s Journal of Psychology, 8, 423–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krumm, G., Lemos, V., & Filippetti, V. A. (2014). Factor structure of the Torrance tests of creative thinking figural Form B in Spanish-speaking children: Measurement invariance across gender. Creativity Research Journal, 26, 72–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, J. T., & Holling, H. (2009). Measurement invariance of divergent thinking across gender, age, and school forms. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 25, 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lan, L., & Kaufman, J. C. (2012). American and Chinese similarities and differences in defining and valuing creative products. Journal of Creative Behavior, 46, 285–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebuda, I., & Karwowski, M. (2013). Tell me your name and I’ll tell you how creative your work is: Author’s name and gender as factors influencing assessment of products’ creativity in four different domains. Creativity Research Journal, 25, 137–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lebuda, I., Zabelina, D. L., & Karwowski, M. (2015). Mind full of ideas: A meta-analysis of the mindfulness–creativity link. Personality and Individual Differences. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.040.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leung, K., Au, A., & Leung, B. W. C. (2004). Creativity and innovation: East–West comparisons with an emphasis on Chinese societies. In S. Lau, A. N. N. Hui, & G. Y. C. Ng (Eds.), Creativity: When East meets West (pp. 113–135). Singapore: World Scientific Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lillard, A. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 3–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lin-Siegler, X., Ahn, J. N., Jondou, C., Fu-Fen, A., & Luna-Lucero, M. (in press). Even Einstein struggled: Effects of learning about great scientists’ struggles on high school students’ motivation to learn science. Journal of Educational Psychology, Feb 11, 2016. doi:10.1037/edu0000092

    Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1994). A collective fear of the collective: Implications for selves and theories of selves. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 568–579.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maslach, C. (1974). Social and personal bases of individuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 29, 411–425.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCrae, R. R., & Costa Jr., P. T. (1997). Personality trait structure as a human universal. American Psychologist, 52, 509–516.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mednick, S. (1962). The associative basis of the creative process. Psychological Review, 69, 220–232.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mohan, J., & Tiwana, M. (1987). Personality and alienation of creative writers: A brief report. Personality and Individual Differences, 8, 449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morling, B., & Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture outside the head: A meta-analysis of individualism–collectivism in cultural products. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12, 199–221.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. R. (2013a). Direct and indirect implications of pathogen prevalence for scientific and technological innovation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45, 971–985.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. R. (2013b). Cultural adaptations to the differential threats posed by hot versus cold climates. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36, 33–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. R., & Schaller, M. (2012). Threat(s) and conformity deconstructed: Perceived threat of infectious disease and its implications for conformist attitudes and behavior. European Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 180–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. R., Trudeau, R., & Schaller, M. (2011). On the origins of cultural differences in conformity: Four tests of the pathogen prevalence hypothesis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 318–329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ng, A. K. (1999). Why Asians are less creative than Westerners. Singapore: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Niu, W., & Sternberg, R. (2002). Contemporary studies on the concept of creativity: The East and the West. Journal of Creative Behavior, 36, 269–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowacki, B. (2012). Western model of creativity: Generalizability and hypothetical consequences for the “non-western world”. International Journal of Creativity and Problem Solving, 23, 13–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowacki, B. (2016). Social representations of creativity: Cultural determinants and educational consequences. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Maria Grzegorzewska University, Warsaw.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nusbaum, E. C., Silvia, P. J., & Beaty, R. E. (2014). Ready, set, create: What instructing people to “be creative” reveals about the meaning and mechanisms of divergent thinking. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 8, 423–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, A. J., Nemeth, C. J., & Akutsu, S. (2013). Consequences of beliefs about the malleability of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 25, 155–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M., & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plucker, J. A. (1999). Is the proof really in the pudding? Reanalysis of Torrance’s longitudinal data. Creativity Research Journal, 12, 103–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plucker, J. A., Beghetto, R. A., & Dow, G. T. (2004). Why isn’t creativity more important to educational psychologist? Potentials, pitfalls, and future directions in creativity research. Educational Psychologist, 39, 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Puente-Diaz, R., Maier, M. A., Brem, A., & Cavazos-Arroyo, J. (2015). Generalizability of the four c model of creativity: A cross-cultural examination of creative perception. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, Oct 19, 2015, doi:10.1037/aca0000038

    Google Scholar 

  • Resnick, M. (2008). Sowing the seeds for a more creative society. Learning & Leading with Technology, 35, 18–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rudowicz, E. (2003). Creativity and culture: A two way interaction. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research, 47, 273–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rudowicz, E. (2004). Creativity among Chinese people: Beyond Western perspective. In S. Lau, A. N. N. Hui, & G. Y. C. Ng (Eds.), Creativity: When East meets West (pp. 55–87). Singapore: World Scientific Publishing.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rudowicz, E., Tokarz, A., & Beauvale, A. (2009). Desirability of personal characteristics associated with creativity: Through the eyes of Polish and Chinese university students. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 4, 104–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (1999a). Creativity need not be social. In A. Montuori & R. Purser (Eds.), Social creativity (Vol. 1, pp. 237–264). Cresskill: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (1999b). The intersection of creativity and culture: Foreword to why Asians are less creative than Westerners. In A. K. Ng (Ed.), Why Asians are less creative than Westerners (pp. ix–xi). Singapore: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (2003). Creativity, cognition, and their educational implications. In J. C. Houtz (Ed.), The educational psychology of creativity (pp. 25–56). Cresskill: Hampton Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Runco, M. A. (2015). A commentary on the social perspective on creativity. Creativity: Theories – Research – Applications, 2, 21–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sawyer, R. K. (2006). Explaining creativity: The science of human innovation. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaller, M., & Murray, D. R. (2008). Pathogens, personality, and culture: Disease prevalence predicts worldwide variability in sociosexuality, extraversion, and openness to experience. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 212–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Silvia, P. J. (2015). Intelligence and creativity are pretty similar after all. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 599–606

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1990). Psychology, science, and history: An introduction to historiometry. New Haven: Yale University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (1991). Personality correlates of exceptional personal influence: A note on Thorndike’s (1950) creators and leaders. Creativity Research Journal, 4, 67–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (2009). Historiometry in personality and social psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 3, 49–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (2012). Taking the US Patent Office creativity criteria seriously: A quantitative three-criterion definition and its implications. Creativity Research Journal, 24, 97–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simonton, D. K. (in press). Creativity and free will: How creative thought enhances personal agency. In M. Karwowski & J. C. Kaufman (Eds.), Creative self. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 607–627.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R., & Lubart, T. (1999). The concept of creativity: Prospect and paradigm. In R. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (pp. 3–15). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. W., & Lee, S. Y. (1990). Context of achievement: A study of American, Chinese, and Japanese children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 55 (1–2, Serial No. 221).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, H. W., & Stigler, J. W. (1992). The learning gap: Why our schools are failing and what we can learn from Japanese and Chinese education. New York: Summit Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sundararajan, L., & Raina, M. K. (2015). Revolutionary creativity, East and West: A critique from indigenous psychology. Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology, 35, 3–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tang, M., Werner, C., Cao, G., Tumasjan, A., Shen, J., Shi, J., et al. (2015). Creative expression and its evaluation on work-related verbal tasks: A comparison of Chinese and German samples. Journal of Creative Behavior. doi:10.1002/jocb.134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tang, M., Werner, C. H., & Karwowski, M. (2016). Differences in creative mindset between Germany and Poland: The mediating effect of individualism and collectivism, Thinking Skills and Creativity, 21, 31–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taras, V., Rowney, J., & Steel, P. (2009). Half a century of measuring culture: Review of approaches, challenges, and limitations based on the analysis of 121 instruments for quantifying culture. Journal of International Management, 15, 357–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1968). Torrance tests of creative thinking. Princeton: Personnel Press, Incorporated.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P. (1988). The nature of creativity as manifest in its testing. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The nature of creativity (pp. 43–73). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torrance, E. P., & Sato, S. (1979). Figural creative thinking abilities of United States and Japanese majors in education. Creative Child & Adult Quarterly, 4, 216–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urban, K. K., & Jellen, H. G. (1996). Test for creative thinking-drawing production (TCT-DP). Lisse: Swets and Zeitlinger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van de Vliert, E. (2008). Climate, affluence, and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenberg, R. J., & Lance, C. E. (2000). A review and synthesis of the measurement invariance literature: Suggestions, practices, and recommendations for organizational research. Organizational Research Methods, 3, 4–70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotski, L. S. (1930). Imagination and creativity in childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 1, 7–97.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallace, D. B., & Gruber, H. E. (1989). Creative people at work: Twelve cognitive case studies. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wallach, M. A., & Kogan, N. (1965). Modes of thinking in young children. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yamagishi, T., & Yamagishi, M. (1994). Trust and commitment in the United States and Japan. Motivation and Emotion, 18, 129–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yang, K. S. (1986). Chinese personality and its change. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The psychology of the Chinese people (pp. 106–170). Hong Kong: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zedelius, C. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2015). Mind wandering “Ahas” versus mindful reasoning: Alternative routes to creative solutions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 834.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The preparation of this chapter was supported by the Polish Ministry of Science and Higher Education (Iuventus Plus Program). I thank Dorota M. Jankowska and Vlad Glăveanu for their assistance in preparing this chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Maciej Karwowski .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Copyright information

© 2016 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Karwowski, M. (2016). Culture and Psychometric Studies of Creativity. In: Glăveanu, V. (eds) The Palgrave Handbook of Creativity and Culture Research. Palgrave Studies in Creativity and Culture. Palgrave Macmillan, London. https://doi.org/10.1057/978-1-137-46344-9_8

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics