Lexical Facility pp 45-65 | Cite as
L2 Word Recognition Skill and Its Measurement
Chapter
First Online:
- 334 Downloads
Abstract
This chapter examines word recognition skill as a key aspect of second language (L2) vocabulary skill and, in turn, a basic element of L2 proficiency. It identifies the role of word recognition skill in text comprehension, describes the lexical decision task (LDT) as a tool for measuring of word recognition skill, and examines LDT performance as a window on word knowledge.
References
- Akamatsu, N. (2003). The effects of first language orthographic features on second language reading in text. Language Learning, 53(2), 207–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Akamatsu, N. (2008). The effects of training on automatization of word recognition in English as a foreign language. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 29(2), 175–193. doi: 10.1017/S0142716408080089.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Andrews, S. (1992). Frequency and neighborhood effects on lexical access: Lexical similarity or orthographic redundancy? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(2), 234–254. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.18.2.234.Google Scholar
- Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision phase. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(3), 340–357. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.10.3.340.Google Scholar
- Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., & Cortese, M. J. (2006). Visual word recognition: The journey from features to meaning (a travel update). In M. J. Traxler & M. A. Gernsbacher (Eds.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (2nd ed., pp. 285–375). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Bell, L. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1994). Reading skill: Some adult comparisons. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 244–255. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Brown, J. D. (2005). Testing in language programs: A comprehensive guide to English language assessment. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
- Carreiras, M., Perea, M., & Grainger, J. (1997). Effects of the orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(4), 857.Google Scholar
- De Groot, A. M., Delmaar, P., & Lupker, S. J. (2000). The processing of interlexical homographs in translation recognition and lexical decision: Support for non-selective access to bilingual memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 53(2), 397–428.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Dijkstra, T. (2005). Bilingual visual word recognition and lexical access. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. de Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 179–201). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Elgort, I. (2013). Effects of L1 definitions and cognate status of test items on the vocabulary size test. Language Testing, 30(2), 253–272. doi: 10.1177/0265532212459028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143–188.Google Scholar
- Favreau, M., & Segalowitz, N. S. (1983). Automatic and controlled processes in the first and second language of reading fluent bilinguals. Memory and Cognition, 11(6), 565–574. doi: 10.3758/BF03198281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fender, M. J. (2001). A review of L1 and L2/ESL word integration development involved in lower-level text processing. Language Learning, 51(2), 319–396. doi: 10.1111/0023-8333.00157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Fodor, J. (1983). Modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
- Geva, E., & Wang, M. (2001). The development of basic reading skills in children: A cross-language perspective. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21, 182–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Harley, T. A. (2013). The psychology of language: From data to theory (4th ed.). Hove: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
- Harrington, M. (2006). The lexical decision task as a measure of L2 lexical proficiency. EUROSLA Yearbook, 6(1), 147–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Holmes, V. M. (2009). Bottom-up processing and reading comprehension in experienced adult readers. Journal of Research in Reading, 32(3), 309–326. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01396.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing, 2(2), 127–160. doi: 10.1007/BF00401799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Hulstijn, J. H., Van Gelderen, A., & Schoonen, R. (2009). Automatization in second language acquisition: What does the coefficient of variation tell us? Applied PsychoLinguistics, 30(04), 555–582.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jacobs, A. M., & Grainger, J. (1994). Models of visual word recognition: Sampling the state of the art. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(6), 1311.Google Scholar
- Jeon, E. H., & Yamashita, J. (2014). L2 reading comprehension and its correlates: A meta-analysis. Language Learning, 64(1), 160–212. doi: 10.1111/lang.12034.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Jiang, N. (2013). Conducting reaction time research in second language studies. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
- Juffs, M., & Harrington, M. (2011). Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching, 44(2), 137–166. doi: 10.1017/S0261444810000509.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1992). A capacity theory comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99(1), 122–149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
- Kintsch, W. (2005). An overview of top-down and bottom-up effects in comprehension: The C-I perspective. Discourse Processes, 39(2–3), 125–128. doi: 10.1080/0163853X.2005.9651676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Koda, K. (1992). The effects of lower-level processing skills on FL reading performance: Implications for instruction. The Modern Language Journal, 76(4), 502–512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Koda, K. (1996). L2 word recognition research: A critical review. The Modern Language Journal, 80(4), 450–460.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Koda, K. (2005). Insights into second language reading: A cross-linguistic approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Koda, K. (2007). Reading and language learning: Crosslinguistic constraints on second language reading development. Language Learning, 57, 1–44. doi: 10.1111/0023-8333.101997010-i1.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Kroll, J., Van Hell, J., Tokowicz, N., & Green, D. (2010). The revised hierarchical model: A critical review and assessment. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(3), 373–381. doi: 10.1017/S136672891000009X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6(2), 293–323. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Lewellen, M. J., Goldinger, S. D., Pisoni, D. B., & Greene, B. G. (1993). Lexical familiarity and processing efficiency: Individual differences in naming, lexical decision, and semantic categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 316–330. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.316.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Luce, R. D. (1986). Response times. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
- Meara, P. (2002). The rediscovery of vocabulary. Second Language Research, 18(4), 393–407. doi: 10.1191/0267658302sr211xx.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Meara, P., Lightbown, P. M., & Halter, R. H. (1994). The effects of cognates on the applicability of yes/no vocabulary tests. The Canadian Modern Language Review, 50(2), 296–311.Google Scholar
- Nassaji, H. (2014). The role and importance of lower-level processes in second language reading. Language Teaching, 47(1), 1–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Pachella, R. G. (1974). The interpretation of reaction time in information processing research. In B. H. Kantowitz (Ed.), Human information processing: Tutorials in performance and cognition (pp. 41–82). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
- Pellicer-Sánchez, A., & Schmitt, S. (2012). Scoring yes-no vocabulary tests: Reaction time vs. nonword approaches. Language Testing, 29(4), 489–509. doi: 10.1177/0265532212438053.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Perfetti, C. A., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 22–37. doi: 10.1080/10888438.2013.827687.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Plonsky, L., & Oswald, F. L. (2014). How big is “big”? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research. Language Learning, 64, 878–912. doi:10.1111/lang. 12079.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Schmitt, N. (2010). Researching vocabulary. A vocabulary research manual. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Segalowitz, N. (2010). Cognitive bases of second language fluency. New York: Routledge.Google Scholar
- Segalowitz, N., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1993). Skilled performance, practice and differentiation of speed-up from automatization effects: Evidence from second language word recognition. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 14(3), 369–385. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400010845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Segalowitz, N., Watson, V., & Segalowitz, S. J. (1995). Vocabulary skill: Single case assessment of automaticity of word recognition in a timed lexical decision task. Second Language Research, 11(2), 121–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Segalowitz, N., Segalowitz, S. J., & Wood, A. G. (1998). Assessing the development of automaticity in second language word recognition. Applied PsychoLinguistics, 19(1), 53–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Shiotsu, T. (2009). Reading ability and components of word recognition speed: The case of L1-Japanese EFL learners. In Z. Han & N. J. Anderson (Eds.), Second language reading research and instruction: Crossing the boundaries (pp. 15–39). Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
- Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Cunningham, A. E. (1991). Beyond phonological processes: Print exposure and orthographic processing. In Phonological processes in literacy: A tribute to Isabelle Y. Liberman (pp. 219–235). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
- Stone, G., & Van Orden, C. (1992). Resolving empirical inconsistencies concerning priming, frequency, and nonword foils in lexical decision. Language and Speech, 35(3), 295–324. doi: 10.1177/002383099203500302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Wagenmakers, E. J., Ratcliff, R., Gomez, P., & McKoon, G. (2008). A diffusion model account of criterion shifts in the lexical decision task. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(1), 140–159. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.04.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
- Yamashita, J. (2013). Word recognition subcomponents and passage level reading in a foreign language. Reading in a Foreign Language, 25(1), 52–71.Google Scholar
- Yap, M., & Balota, D. (2015). Visual word recognition. In A. Pollastsek & R. Treiman (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of reading (pp. 26–43). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Copyright information
© The Author(s) 2018