Advertisement

From Relational Ontology to Transformative Activist Stance on Development and Learning: Expanding Vygotsky’s (CHAT) Project

  • Anna Stetsenko
Chapter
  • 329 Downloads
Part of the Marxism and Education book series (MAED)

Abstract

Research in psychology and education today is going through a paradoxical phase, perhaps to such an extent that the cliché “the best of times, the worst of times” cannot be avoided when trying to describe it. On the one hand, we are witnessing much ferment and enthusiasm as novel ideas, exciting discoveries, and innovative methodologies are emerging and flourish across a variety of approaches that explore the effects of culture and society on human development. These new and innovative approaches are often underwritten by a common commitment to social justice and equity (these approaches will be termed sociocultural herein for lack of a better unifying term). On the other hand, it is impossible not to notice a rising tide, indeed a tsunami, of starkly mechanistic views that reduce human development (more boldly now than at any other time in recent history) to processes in the brain rigidly constrained by genetic blueprints passed on to contemporary humans from the dawn of the evolution. The sad irony is that these latter views represent a strikingly united front in sharp ascendance—drawing together resurrected tenets of sociobiology, innatist linguistics, narrowly conceived neuroscience, orthodox modular cognitivism, with the test-and-control, knowledge transmission–based educational models following suit—while the alternative sociocultural approaches remain starkly disconnected, without much dialogue or coordination among them. Indeed, no consensus of a sort now propagated by the “new” reductionist synthesis is apparent in sociocultural approaches that are scattered across areas as diverse as critical pedagogy, social theory, adult learning, disability studies, critical race theory, constructivist education, science studies, human-computer interaction, feminist studies, literary criticism, cultural anthropology, and developmental psychology, among others.

Keywords

Human Development Human Nature Human Subjectivity Critical Pedagogy Critical Race Theory 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Altman, I. and B. Rogoff (1987) World Views in Psychology: Trait, Interactional, Organismic, and Transactional Perspectives. In D. Stokolis and I. Altman (eds.) Handbook of Environmental Psychology, 1–40 (New York: Wiley).Google Scholar
  2. Arievitch, I.M. and A. Stetsenko (2000) The Quality of Cultural Tools and Cognitive Development: Gal’perin’s Perspective and its Implications. Human Development, 43: 69–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Barab, S.A. and Roth, W.M. (2006). Curriculum-Based Ecosystems: Supporting Knowing from an Ecological Perspective. Educational Researcher, 35: 3–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Clark, A. (1997) Being There: Putting Brain, Body and World Together Again (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
  5. Collis, B. and J. Moonen (2001). Flexible Learning in a Digital World: Experiences and Expectations (London: Kogan Page).Google Scholar
  6. Diggins, J.P. (1994). The Promise of Pragmatism: Modernism and the Crisis of Knowledge and Authority (Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press).Google Scholar
  7. Eisenhart, M. (2001). Educational Ethnography. Past, Present, and Future: Ideas to Think with. Educational Researcher, 30: 16–27.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Garrison, J. (1995) (ed.) The New Scholarship on Dewey (Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic).Google Scholar
  9. Gergen, K.J. (2001). Psychological Science in a Postmodern Context. American Psychologist, 56: 803–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Gibson, J.J. (1966) The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems (Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin).Google Scholar
  11. Glass, R.D. (2001) On Paulo Freire’s Philosophy of Praxis and the Foundation of Liberation Education. Educational Researcher, 30: 15–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Gottlieb, G. (2003) On Making Behavioral Genetics Truly Developmental. Human Development, 46, 337–55.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Harding, S. (2004) The Feminist Standpoint Theory Reader: Intellectual and Political Controversies (New York: Routledge).Google Scholar
  14. Harré, R. (2002) Public Sources of the Personal Mind: Social Constructionism in Context. Theory and Psychology, 12: 611–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Hicks, D. (2000) Self and Other in Bakhtin’s Early Philosophical Essays: Prelude to a Theory of Prose Consciousness. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 7: 227–42.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Holland, D., W. Lachicotte, Jr., D. Skinner, and C. Cain (2001) Identity and Agency in Cultural Worlds (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).Google Scholar
  17. Holzman, L. (2006) Activating Postmodernism. Theory and Psychology, 16: 109–23.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Hutchins, E. (1995) Cognition in the Wild (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).Google Scholar
  19. Ingold, T. (2000) Perception of the Environment: Essays in Livelihood, Dwelling and Skill (London: Routledge Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. John-Steiner, V. (1999) Sociocultural and Feminist Theory: Mutuality and Relevance. In S. Chaiklin, M. Hedegaard, and U. J. Jensen (eds.) Activity Theory and Social Practice, 201–44 (Aarhus, Denmark: Aarhus University Press).Google Scholar
  21. Kestenbaum, V. (1977) The Phenomenological Sense of John Dewey (Atlantic Highlands, NJ: Humanities Press).Google Scholar
  22. Kitchener, R.F. (1996) The Nature of the Social for Piaget and Vygotsky. Human Development, 39: 243–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Knox, J.E. (1993) Introduction. In L.S. Vygotsky and A.R. Luria (eds.) Studies on the History of Behavior: Ape, Primitive, and Child, 1–35 (London: Lawrence Erlbaum).Google Scholar
  24. Latour, B. (1987). Science in Action: How to Follow Scientists and Engineers through Society (Milton Keynes: Open University Press).Google Scholar
  25. Lave, J. and E. Wenger (1991). Situated Learning: Legitimate Peripheral Participation (New York: Cambridge University Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Lickliter, R. and H. Honeycutt (2003) Developmental Dynamics: Toward a Biologically Plausible Evolutionary Psychology. Psychological Bulletin, 129: 819–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Marx, K. (1978) Theses on Feuerbach. In R.C. Tucker (ed.) The Marx-Engels Reader (2nd Edition), 143–145 (New York: W.W. Norton).Google Scholar
  28. Müller, U. and J.I.M. Carpendale (2000) The Role of Social Interaction in Piaget’s Theory: Language for Social Cooperation and Social Cooperation for Language. New Ideas in Psychology, 18: 139–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Newman, F. and L. Holzman, (1993) Lev Vygotsky: Revolutionary Scientist (Florence: Taylor and Frances/Routledge).Google Scholar
  30. Overton, W.F. (1997) Beyond Dichotomy: An Embodied Active Agent for Cultural Psychology. Culture and Psychology, 3: 315–34.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Piaget, J. (1977/1995) Sociological Studies (London: Routledge). (Original work published in 1977).Google Scholar
  32. Sfard, A. (1998) On Two Metaphors for Learning and the Dangers of Choosing Just One. Educational Researcher, 27: 4–13.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Stetsenko, A. (2004) Introduction to Vygotsky’s “Tool and Sign in Child Development.” In R. Rieber and D. Robbins (eds.) Essential Vygotsky, 499–510 (New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum).Google Scholar
  34. Stetsenko, A. (2005) Activity as Object-Related: Resolving the Dichotomy of Individual and Collective Planes of Activity. Mind, Culture, and Activity, 12: 70–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. Stetsenko, A. and I. M Arievitch (2004) Vygotskian Collaborative Project of Social Transformation: History, Politics, and Practice in Knowledge Construction. The International Journal of Critical Psychology, 12: 58–80.Google Scholar
  36. Thompson, E. and F. J Varela (2001) Radical Embodiment: Neural Dynamics and Consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5: 418–25.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Vianna, E. and A. Stetsenko (2006) Embracing History through Transforming It: Contrasting Piagetian versus Vygotskian (Activity) Theories of Learning and Development to Expand Constructivism within a Dialectical View of History. Theory and Psychology, 16: 81–108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Vygotsky, L.S. (1997) The History of the Development of Higher Mental Functions. In R. Rieber (ed.) The Collected Works of L.S. Vygotsky (Vol. 4), 1–278 (New York: Plenum).Google Scholar
  39. Vygotsky, L.S. (2004a) Thinking and Speech. In R. Rieber and D. Robinson (eds.) Essential Vygotsky, 33–148 (New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum).Google Scholar
  40. Vygotsky, L.S. (2004b) Fundamentals of Defectology. In R. Rieber and D. Robinson (eds.), Essential Vygotsky, 153–199 (New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum).Google Scholar
  41. Wertsch, J. (2005) Essay Review of “Making of Human Beings: Bioecological Perspectives on Human Development” by U. Bronfenbrenner. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23: 143–51.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Peter E. Jones 2011

Authors and Affiliations

  • Anna Stetsenko

There are no affiliations available

Personalised recommendations