Advertisement

The Marine Mammal Captivity Issue: Time for a Paradigm Shift

  • Lori Marino
Chapter
Part of the The Palgrave Macmillan Animal Ethics Series book series (PMAES)

Abstract

Dolphin and whale (cetacean) captivity is one of the most contentious cultural issues of modern times. But is there a way to proceed to resolve this issue based on evidence for whether cetaceans can thrive in marine parks and aquariums? By asking “who are cetaceans?” we gain insight into why their natural characteristics as wide-ranging, large brained and socially complex mammals make them particularly vulnerable to the stresses of living in concrete tanks and performing. The result is a growing body of scientific evidence for physical and psychological damage in cetaceans kept in concrete tanks. The time is ripe for the “captivity industry” and cetacean advocates and scientists to come together to create a paradigm shift in how we relate to cetaceans from exploitation to one of recognizing their need to be in their natural environment.

Bibliography

  1. Abramson, J. Z., V. Hernandez-Lloreda, J. Call, and F. Colmenares. “Experimental Evidence for Action Imitation in Killer Whales (Orcinus orca).” Animal Cognition 16, no. 1 (2013): 11–22.Google Scholar
  2. Allman, J. M., K. K. Watson, N. A. Tetrault, and A. Y. Hakeem. “Intuition and Autism: A Possible Role for Von Economo Neurons.” Trends in Cognitive Science 9 (2005): 367–73.Google Scholar
  3. Baron-Cohen, S. “Radical Behaviorism.” Edge. Accessed June 23, 2015. http://edge.org/response-detail/25473.
  4. Bigg, M. A., P. E. Olesiuk, G. M. Ellis, J. K. Ford, and K. C. Balcomb. “Social Organization and Genealogy of Resident Killer Whales (Orcinus Orca) in the Coastal Waters of British Columbia and Washington State.” In “Individual Recognition of Cetaceans: Use of Photo-Identification and Other Techniques to Estimate Population Parameters,” edited by P. S. Hammond, S. A. Mizroch, and G. P. Donovan. Reports of the International Whaling Commission Special Issue 12: 383–405.Google Scholar
  5. Ceta-Base. “Lugalogue.” Accessed August 4, 2014. http://www.ceta-base.com/lugalogue/ddl/ddl_nmmp.html.
  6. Connor, R. C., M. R. Heithaus, and L. M. Barre. “Superalliance of Bottlenose Dolphins.” Nature 397 (1999): 571–72.Google Scholar
  7. Connor, R. C., A. F. Richards, R. A. Smolker, and J. Mann. “Patterns of Female Attractiveness in Indian Ocean Bottlenose Dolphins.” Behaviour 133 (1996): 37–69.Google Scholar
  8. Connor, R. C., R. A. Smolker, and A. F. Richards. “Two Levels of Alliance Formation among Male Bottlenosed Dolphins (Tursiops sp.).” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 89 (1992): 987–90.Google Scholar
  9. Deecke, V. B., J. K. Ford, and P. Spong. “Dialect Change in Resident Killer Whales: Implications for Vocal Learning and Cultural Transmission.” Animal Behaviour 40 (2000): 629–38.Google Scholar
  10. Defran, R. H., and K. Pryor. “The Behavior and Training of Cetaceans in Captivity.” In Cetacean Behavior: Mechanisms and Functions, edited by L. Herman, 319–64. New York: Wiley, 1980.Google Scholar
  11. Delfour, F., and K. Marten. “Mirror Image Processing in Three Marine Mammal Species: Killer Whales (Orcinus orca), False Killer Whales (Pseudorca crassidens), and California Sea Lions (Zalophus californianus).” Behavioral Processes 53 (2001): 181–90.Google Scholar
  12. DeMaster, D. P., and J. K. Drevenak. “Survivorship Patterns in Three Species of Captive Cetaceans.” Marine Mammal Science 4, no. 4 (1998): 297–311.Google Scholar
  13. Duffield, D. A., and R. S. Wells. “The Combined Application of Chromosome, Protein, and Molecular Data for Investigation of Social Unit Structure and Dynamics in Tursiops truncatus.” In “Genetic Ecology of Whales and Dolphins,” edited by A. R. Hoelzel. Reports of the International Whaling Commission Special Issue 13 (1991): 155–69.Google Scholar
  14. Fish, M. P., and W. H. Mowbray. “Production of Underwater Sound by the White Whale or Beluga, Delphinapterus leucas (Pallus).” Journal of Marine Research 20 (1962): 149–61.Google Scholar
  15. Foote, A. D., R. M. Griffin, D. Howitt, L. Larrson, P. J. O. Miller, and A. R. Hoelzel. “Killer Whales Are Capable of Vocal Learning.” Biology Letters 2, no. 4 (2006): 509–12.Google Scholar
  16. Ford, J. K. B. “Acoustic Behaviour of Resident Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia.” Canadian Journal of Zoology 67 (1989): 727–45.Google Scholar
  17. ———. “Killer Whale (Orcinus orca).” In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, edited by W. F. Perrin, B. Wursig, and J. G. M. Thewissen, 650–57.Google Scholar
  18. Ford, J. K., G. M. Ellis, and K. C. Balcomb. Killer Whales. Vancouver: University of British Columbia Press, 2000.Google Scholar
  19. Fowler, M. E. “A Stereotyped Behavior Pattern in Dolphins.” In Zoo and Wild Animal Medicine, edited by M. E. Fowler, 33–34. Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 1978.Google Scholar
  20. Gaskin, D. E. The Ecology of Whales and Dolphins. London: Heinemann, 1982.Google Scholar
  21. Gazda, S. K., R. C. Connor, R. K. Edgar, and F. Cox. “A Division of Labor with Role Specialization in Group-Hunting Bottlenosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) off Cedar Key, Florida.” Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B 272 (2005): 135–40.Google Scholar
  22. Geisler, J. H., M. R. McGowen, G. Yang, and J. Gatesy. “A Supermatrix Analysis of Genomic, Morphological, and Paleontological Data from Crown Cetacea.” BMC Evolutionary Biology 11 (2011): 112. doi:10.1186/1471-2148-11-112.
  23. Greenwood, A. G. “A Stereotyped Behavior Pattern in Dolphins.” Aquatic Mammals 5 (1977): 15–17.Google Scholar
  24. Hastie, G. D., B. Wilson, and P. M. Thompson. “Diving Deep in a Foraging Hotspot: Acoustic Insights into Bottlenose Dolphin Dive Depths and Feeding Behaviour.” Marine Biology 148 (2006): 1181–88.Google Scholar
  25. Herman, L. M. “Cognition and Language Competencies of Bottlenosed Dolphins.” In Dolphin Cognition and Behavior: A Comparative Approach, edited by R. J. Schusterman, J. Thomas, and F. G. Wood, 221–51. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1986.Google Scholar
  26. ———. “Intelligence and Rational Behaviour in the Bottlenosed Dolphin.” In Rational Animals? edited by S. Hurley and M. Nudds, 439–68. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2006.Google Scholar
  27. ———. “Vocal, Social, and Self-Imitation by Bottlenosed Dolphins.” In Imitation in Animals and Artifacts, edited by K. Dautenhahn and C. L. Nehaniv, 63–108. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2002.Google Scholar
  28. Herman, L. M., D. S. Matus, E. Y. K. Herman, M. Ivancic, and A. A. Pack. “The Bottlenosed Dolphin’s (Tursiops truncatus) Understanding of Gestures as Symbolic Representations of Its Body Parts.” Animal Learning and Behavior 29 (2001): 250–64.Google Scholar
  29. Hof, P. R., R. Chanis, and L. Marino. “Cortical Complexity in Cetacean Brains.” The Anatomical Record 287A (2005): 1142–52.Google Scholar
  30. Hof, P. R., and E. Van Der Gucht. “The Structure of the Cerebral Cortex of the Humpback Whale, Megaptera novaeangliae (Cetacea, Mysticeti, Balaenopteridae).” The Anatomical Record 290 (2007): 1–31.Google Scholar
  31. Hooper, S. L., D. R. Reiss, M. Carter, and B. McCowan. “Importance of Contextual Saliency on Vocal Imitation by Bottlenose Dolphins.” International Journal of Comparative Psychology 19 (2006): 116–28.Google Scholar
  32. Jacobs, M. S., W. L. McFarland, and P. J. Morgane. “The Anatomy of the Brain of the Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus): Rhinic Lobe (Rhinencephalon), the Archicortex.” Brain Research Bulletin 4 (1979): 1–108.Google Scholar
  33. Jerison, H. J. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York: Academic Press, 1973.Google Scholar
  34. Jett, J. S., and J. M. Ventre. “Keto and Tilikum Express the Stress of Orca Captivity.” The Orca Project, January 20, 2011. https://theorcaproject.wordpress.com/2011/01/20/keto-tilikum-express-stress-of-orca-captivity/.
  35. ———. “Orca (Orca orcinus) Captivity and Vulnerability to Mosquito-Transmitted Viruses.” Journal of Marine Animals and Their Ecology 5, no. 2 (2012): 9–16.Google Scholar
  36. Kesarev, V. S. “The Inferior Brain of the Dolphin.” Soviet Science Review 2 (1971): 52–58.Google Scholar
  37. Krützen, M., J. Mann, M. R. Heithaus, R. C. Connor, L. Bejder, and W. B. Sherwin. “Cultural Transmission of Tool Use in Bottlenose Dolphins.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 102, no. 25 (2005): 8939–43.Google Scholar
  38. Leung, E. S., V. Vergara, and L. G. Barrett-Lennard. “Allonursing in Captive Belugas (Delphinapterus leucas).” Zoo Biology 29 (2010): 633–37.Google Scholar
  39. Lockyer, C., A. Hohn, D. W. Doidge, M. P. Heide-Jørgensen, and R. Suydam. “Age Determination in Belugas (Delphinapterus leucas): A Quest for Validation of Dentinal Layering.” Marine Mammals 33, no. 3 (2007): 293–304.Google Scholar
  40. Lusseau, D.. “Evidence for Social Role in a Dolphin Social Network.” Evolutionary Ecology 21 (2007): 357–66.Google Scholar
  41. Lusseau, D., and L. Conradt. “The Emergence of Unshared Consensus Decisions in Bottlenose Dolphins.” Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 63 (2009): 1067–77.Google Scholar
  42. Lydersen, C., A. R. Martin, K. M. Kovacs, and I. Gjertz. “Summer and Autumn Movements of White Whales (Delphinapterus leucas) in Svalbard, Norway.” Marine Ecology Progress Series 219 (2001): 265–74.Google Scholar
  43. Manger, P. R. “An Examination of Cetacean Brain Structure with a Novel Hypothesis Correlating Thermogenesis to the Evolution of a Big Brain.” Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 81 (2006): 293–338.Google Scholar
  44. Mann, J., B. L. Sargeant, and M. Minor. “Calf Inspection of Fish Catches: Opportunities for Oblique Social Learning.” Marine Mammal Science 24 (2007): 749–73.Google Scholar
  45. Marine Mammal Inventory Report. National Marine Fisheries Service, Office of Protected Resources, 2010.Google Scholar
  46. Marino, L. “Brain Size Evolution.” In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, edited by W. F. Perrin, B. Wursig, and J. G. M. Thewissen, 149–52. San Diego: Academic Press, 2009.Google Scholar
  47. ———. “Brain Structure and Intelligence in Cetaceans.” In Whales and Dolphins: Cognition, Culture, Conservation and Human Perceptions, edited by P. Brakes and M. P. Simmonds, 115–28. London: Earthscan, 2011.Google Scholar
  48. ———. “A Comparison of Encephalization between Odontocete Cetaceans and Anthropoid Primates.” Brain, Behavior, and Evolution 51 (1998): 230–38.Google Scholar
  49. Marino, L., C. Butti, R. C. Connor, R. E. Fordyce, L. M. Herman, P. R. Hof, L. Lefebvre, et al. “A Claim in Search of Evidence: Reply to Manger’s Thermogenesis Hypothesis of Cetacean Brain Structure.” Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society 83 (2008): 417–40.Google Scholar
  50. Marino, L., and T. Frohoff. “Towards a New Paradigm of Non-Captive Research on Cetacean Cognition.” PLoS ONE 6, no. 9 (2011): e24121. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024121.
  51. Marino, L., D. W. McShea, and M. Uhen. “Origin and Evolution of Large Brains in Toothed Whales.” The Anatomical Record Part A 281A (2004): 1247–55.Google Scholar
  52. McCowan, B., L. Marino, E. Vance, L. Walke, and D. Reiss. “Bubble Ring Play of Bottlenose Dolphins: Implications for Cognition.” Journal of Comparative Psychology 114 (2000): 98–106.Google Scholar
  53. Montgomery, S. H., J. Geisler, M. R. McGowan, C. Fox, L. Marino, and J. Gatesy. “The Evolutionary History of Cetacean Brain and Body Size.” Evolution 67, no. 11 (2013): 3339–53.Google Scholar
  54. Murayama, T., Y. Fujii, T. Hashimoto, A. Shimoda, S. Iijima, K. Hayasaka, N. Shiroma, et al. “Preliminary Study of Object Labeling Using Sound Production in a Beluga.” International Journal of Comparative Psychology 25 (2012): 195–207.Google Scholar
  55. Murayama, T., S. Iijima, H. Katsumata, and K. Arai. “Imitation in a Beluga.” International Journal of Comparative Psychology 27, no. 3 (2014): 369–84.Google Scholar
  56. Noda, K., H. Akiyoshi, M. Aoki, T. Shimada, and F. Ohashi. “Relationship between Transportation Stress and Polymorphonuclear Cell Functions of Bottlenose Dolphins, Tursiops truncatus.” Journal of Veterinary Medical Science 69, no. 4 (2007): 379–83.Google Scholar
  57. Nussbaum, M. C. “The Capabilities Approach and Animal Entitlements.” In The Oxford Handbook of Animal Ethics, edited by T. L. Beauchamp and R. G. Frey, 228–51. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 2011.Google Scholar
  58. O’Corry-Crowe, G. M. “Beluga Whale.” In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, edited by W. F. Perrin, B. Wursig, and J. G. M. Thewissen, 108–12. San Diego: Academic Press, 2009.Google Scholar
  59. Oelschlager, H. A., and J. S. Oelschlager. “Brains.” In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, edited by W. F. Perrin, B. Wursig, and J. G. M. Thewissen, 134–58. San Diego: Academic Press, 2009.Google Scholar
  60. Olesiuk, P., M. Bigg, and G. M. Ellis. “Life History and Population Dynamics of Resident Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) in the Coastal Waters of British Columbia and Washington State.” In “Individual Recognition of Cetaceans: Use of Photo-Identification and Other Techniques to Estimate Population Parameters,” edited by P. S. Hammond, S. A. Mizroch, and G. P. Donovan. Reports of the International Whaling Commission Special Issue 12: 209–44.Google Scholar
  61. The Orca Project. “Deceased Orcas by Date.” Last updated December 5, 2013. http://theorcaproject.wordpress.com/killer-whale-orca-database/killer-whale-orca-dead-date/.
  62. Pack, A. A., and L. M. Herman. “The Dolphin’s (Tursiops truncatus) Understanding of Human Gaze and Pointing: Knowing What and Where.” Journal of Comparative Psychology 121 (2007): 34–45.Google Scholar
  63. Perrin, W. F., B. Wursig, and J. G. M. Thewissen, eds. Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals. San Diego: Academic Press, 2009.Google Scholar
  64. Reiss, D., and L. Marino. “Self-Recognition in the Bottlenose Dolphin: A Case of Cognitive Convergence.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 98 (2001): 5937–42.Google Scholar
  65. Reiss, D., and B. McCowan. “Spontaneous Vocal Mimicry and Production by Bottlenosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus): Evidence for Vocal Learning.” Journal of Comparative Psychology 107 (1993): 301–12.Google Scholar
  66. Rendell, L. E., and H. Whitehead. “Culture in Whales and Dolphins.” Behavioural and Brain Sciences 24 (2001): 309–24.Google Scholar
  67. Richard, P. R., M. P. Heide-Jorgensen, J. R. Orr, R. Dietz, and T. G. Smith. “Summer and Autumn Movements and Habitat Use by Belugas in the Canadian High Arctic and Adjacent Areas.” Arctic 54 (2001): 207–22.Google Scholar
  68. Richards, D. G., J. P. Wolz, and L. M. Herman. “Vocal Mimicry of Computer Generated Sounds and Vocal Labeling of Objects by a Bottlenosed Dolphin, Tursiops truncatus.” Journal of Comparative Psychology 98 (1984): 10–28.Google Scholar
  69. Ridgway, S., D. Carder, M. Jeffries, and M. Todd. “Spontaneous Human Speech Mimicry by a Cetacean.” Current Biology 22, no. 20 (2012): R860–R861.Google Scholar
  70. Ridgway, S. H., and R. H. Brownson. “Relative Brain Sizes and Cortical Surface Areas in Odontocetes.” Acta Zoologica Fennica 172 (1984): 149–52.Google Scholar
  71. Rose, N. A., E. C. M. Parsons, and R. Farinato. The Case against Marine Mammals in Captivity. Washington, DC: Humane Society of the United States, 2009.Google Scholar
  72. Sargeant, B. L., and Janet Mann. “Developmental Evidence for Foraging Traditions in Wild Bottlenose Dolphins.” Animal Behaviour 78 (2009): 715–21.Google Scholar
  73. Schevill, W. E., and B. Lawrence. “Underwater Listening to the White Porpoise (Delphinapterus leucas).” Science 109 (1949): 143–44.Google Scholar
  74. Small, R. J., and D. P. DeMaster. “Survival of Five Species of Captive Marine Mammals.” Marine Mammal Science 11, no. 2 (1995): 209–26.Google Scholar
  75. Smith, J. D., J. Schull, J. Strote, K. McGee, R. Egnor, and L. Erb. “The Uncertain Response in the Bottlenose Dolphin (Tursiops truncatus).” Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 124 (1995): 391–408.Google Scholar
  76. Spoon, T. R., and T. A. Romano. “Neuroimmunological Response of Beluga Whales (Delphinapterus leucas) to Translocation and a Novel Social Environment.” Brain, Behavior and Immunity 26, no 1 (2011): 122–31.Google Scholar
  77. St. Aubin, D. J., and J. R. Geraci. “Capture and Handling Stress Suppresses Circulating Levels of Thyroxine (T4) and Triiodothyronine (T3) in Beluga Whales Delphinapterus leucas.” Physiological Zoology 61, no. 2 (1988): 170–75.Google Scholar
  78. Stewart, B. S. “Killer Whale (Orcinus orca).” In Encyclopedia of Marine Mammals, edited by W. F. Perrin, B. Wursig, and J. G. M. Thewissen, 321–26. San Diego: Academic Press, 2009.Google Scholar
  79. Stewart, K. L., and L. Marino. Dolphin–Human Interaction Programs: Policies, Problems, and Practical Alternatives. Policy paper for Animals and Society Institute, 2009.Google Scholar
  80. Stewart, R. E. A., S. E. Campana, C. M. Jones, and B. E. Stewart. “Bomb Radiocarbon Dating Calibrates Beluga (Delphinapterus leucas) Age Estimates.” Canadian Journal of Zoology 84 (2006): 1840–52.Google Scholar
  81. Sweeney, J. C. “Specific Pathologic Behavior in Aquatic Mammals: Self-Inflicted Trauma.” Soundings: Newsletter of the International Marine Animal Trainers Association 13, no. 1 (1988): 7.Google Scholar
  82. Van Bressem, M.-F., et al. “Epidemiological Pattern of Tattoo Skin Disease: A Potential General Health Indicator for Cetaceans.” Diseases of Aquatic Organisms 85 (2009): 225–37.Google Scholar
  83. Wells, R. S., and M. D. Scott. “Estimating Bottlenose Dolphin Population Parameters from Individual Identification and Capture-Release Techniques.” In “Individual Recognition of Cetaceans: Use of Photo-Identification and Other Techniques to Estimate Population Parameters,” edited by P. S. Hammond, S. A. Mizroch, and G. P. Donovan. Reports of the International Whaling Commission Special Issue 12 (1990): 407–15.Google Scholar
  84. White, T. I. “Whales, Dolphins and Ethics: A Primer.” In Dolphin Communication and Cognition: Past, Present, Future, edited by D. L. Herzing and C. M. Johnson. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2015.Google Scholar
  85. Whitehead, H. “The Culture of Whales and Dolphins.” In Whales and Dolphins: Cognition, Culture, Conservation and Human Perceptions, edited by P. Brakes and M. P. Simmonds, 149–65. London: Earthscan, 2011.Google Scholar
  86. Williams, R., and D. Lusseau. “A Killer Whale Social Network Is Vulnerable to Targeted Removals.” Biology Letters 2, no. 4 (2006): 497–500.Google Scholar
  87. Williams, V. Captive Orcas: Dying to Entertain You. Chippenham, UK: Whale and Dolphin Conservation Society, 2001.Google Scholar
  88. Wise, S. M. “Entitling Non-Human Animals to Fundamental Legal Rights on the Basis of Practical Autonomy.” In Animals, Ethics and Trade: The Challenge of Animal Sentience, edited by J. Turner and J. D’Silva, 87–100. Sterling, VA: Earthscan, 2005.Google Scholar
  89. Woodley, T. H., J. L. Hannah, and D. M. Lavigne. “A Comparison of Survival Rates for Captive and Free-Ranging Bottlenose Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus), Killer Whales (Orcinus orca) and Beluga Whales (Delphinapterus leucas).” International Marine Mammal Association Technical Report 97–02. 1997.Google Scholar
  90. Xitco, M. J., J. D. Gory, and S. A. Kuczaj II. “Dolphin Pointing Is Linked to the Attentional Behavior of a Receiver.” Animal Cognition 7 (2004): 231–38.Google Scholar
  91. ———. “Spontaneous Pointing by Bottlenosed Dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).” Animal Cognition 4 (2001): 115–23.Google Scholar
  92. Yurk, H. “Do Killer Whales Have Culture?” In Animal Social Complexity: Intelligence, Culture, and Individualized Societies, edited by F. B. M. de Waal and P. L. Tyack, 465–67. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2003.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© The Author(s) 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  • Lori Marino
    • 1
  1. 1.Whale Sanctuary ProjectKanabUSA

Personalised recommendations