Abstract
Surprisingly, the study of human voice evolution has long been conducted without any reference to its biological function. Yet, following Darwin’s original concept, John Ohala was the first linguist to assume the functional role of sexual selection to explain vocal dimorphism in humans. Nevertheless, it is only at the very beginning of the millennial that the study of voice attractiveness developed, revealing that beyond its linguistic role, voice also conveys important psycho-socio-biological information that have a significant effect on the speaker’s mating and reproductive success. In this review article, our aim is to synthesize 20 years of research dedicated to the study of vocal preferences and to present the evolutionary benefits associated with such preferences.
Keywords
- Vocal preferences
- Perception
- Language evolution
- Sexual selection
- Evolutionary biology
- Acoustics
- Voice
- Fundamental frequency
- Formant dispersion
- Voice attractiveness
This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution.
Buying options
Notes
- 1.
Runaway selection is a mechanism whereby a secondary sexual trait expressed in one sex is correlated with a preference for the trait in the other sex. The genetic coupling of the trait and the preference leads to self-reinforcing loops of coevolution between the trait and preference for the trait (Travers, 2017).
- 2.
The WHR has been used as an indicator of health and the risk of developing serious health conditions. WHR correlates with fertility (with different optimal values in males and females). The concept and significance of WHR as an indicator of attractiveness has been theorized by Singh (1993) who argued the WHR is a consistent estrogen marker, and thus a reliable proxy of fertility. Women with a 0.7 WHR are usually rated as more attractive by men from Indo-European cultures (Singh & Young 2001), but preferences may vary according to the culture under study (Fisher & Voracek, 2006).
- 3.
At the physiological level, low HNR values are believed to be related to insufficient vocal fold adduction during the so-called “closed” interval of the phonatory cycle. Insufficient closure would allow excessive airflow through the glottis, giving rise to a turbulence noise component in the quasi-periodic source signal. This friction noise would result in a higher noise level in the spectrum, especially in the higher frequencies.
- 4.
The theory of immunocompetence handicap (Zahavi, 1975) suggests that androgen-mediated traits accurately signal condition due to the immunosuppressive effects of androgens. This immunosuppression may be either because testosterone alters the allocation of limited resources between the development of ornamental traits and the immune system or because heightened immune system activity has a propensity to launch autoimmune attacks against gametes, such that suppression of the immune system enhances fertility. Therefore, only healthy individuals can afford to suppress their immune system by raising their testosterone levels, which also augments secondary sexual traits and displays (among which low deep voices for men).
- 5.
Secondary sex characteristics are features that appear during puberty in humans, and at sexual maturity in other animals. Secondary sex characteristics include, for example, the manes of male lions, the bright facial and rump coloration of male mandrills, and horns in many goats and/or antelopes. In humans, visible secondary sex characteristics include pubic hair, enlarged breasts and widened hips of females, facial hair, Adam’s apples on males, etc.
- 6.
In a famous article dealing with vocal communication in animals, Morton (1977) introduces his ‘motivation-structural rules’ theory, which suggests physical proprieties of acoustic signals (sounds of high versus low frequencies) are motivated since they reflect the vocalizer’s body size and inform about his/her intentions and/or emotional state. He argues a large number of birds and mammals use low-frequency sounds to express hostility, threat, and aggression whereas high-frequency sounds are rather used to express fear, submission, and “amicability.”.
References
Aben, P., Pflügera, L., Koppensteiner, M., Coquerellee, M., & Grammer, K. (2015). Sound of female shape: A redundant signal of vocal and facial attractiveness. Evolution and Human Behavior, 36(3), 174–181.
Abitbol, J., Abitbol, P., & Abitbol, B. (1999). Sex hormones and the female voice. Journal of Voice, 13, 424–446.
Amir, O., & Biron-Shental, T. (2003). The impact of hormonal fluctuations on female vocal folds. Current Opinion in Otolaryngology & Head and Neck Surgery, 12, 180–184.
Apicella, C. L., Feinberg, D. R., & Marlowe, F. W. (2007). Voice pitch predicts reproductive success in male hunter-gatherers. Biology Letters, 3(6), 682–684.
Arak, A., & Enquist, M. (1993). Hidden preferences and the evolution of signals. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 340, 207–213. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1993.0059.
Avery, J. D., & Liss, J. M. (1996). Acoustic characteristics of less masculine-sounding male speech. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 99, 3738–3748.
Babel, M., McGuire, G., & King, J. (2014). Towards a more nuanced view of vocal attractiveness. PLoS One, 9(2), e88616.
Bancroft, J. (2005). The endocrinology of sexual arousal. Journal of Endocrinology, 186, 411–427.
Barkat-Defradas, M., Busseuil, C., Chauvy, O., Hirsch, F., Fauth, C., Revis, J., & Amy de la Bretéque, B. (2012). Dimension esthétique des voix normales et dysphoniques: Approches perceptive et acoustique. TIPA 28.
Barkat-Defradas, M., Fauth, C., Didirkova, F., Amy de la Breteque, B., Hirsch, F., Dodane, C., & Sauvage, J. (2015). Dysphonia is beautiful: A perceptual and acoustic analysis of vocal roughness. International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, 18th ICPhS, Glasgow 10–14 August 2015, Scotland, UK.
Borkowska, B., & Pawlowski, B. (2011). Female voice frequency in the context of dominance and attractiveness perception. Animal Behaviour, 82(1), 55–59.
Bovet, J. (2018). The evolution of feminine beauty. In Z. Kapoula et al. (Eds.), Exploring Trans disciplinarity in Art and Sciences (pp. 327–348). Springer International Publishing AG, Part of Springer Nature.
Bovet, J. (2019). Evolutionary theories and men’s preferences for women’s waist-to-hip ratio: Which hypotheses remain? A Systematic Review. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01221.
Bovet, J., & Raymond, M. (2015). Preferred women’s waist-to-hip ratio variation over the last 2, 500 years. PLoS One, 10, e0123284.
Bruckert, L., Bestelmeyer, P., Latinus, M., Rouger, J., Charest, I., Rousselet, G. A., et al. (2010). Vocal attractiveness increases by averaging. Current Biology, 20(2), 116–120.
Bruckert, L., Lienard, J.-S., Lacroix, A., Kreutzer, M., & Leboucher, G. (2006). Women use voice parameters to assess men’s characteristics. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 273(1582), 83–89.
Bryant, Gregory A., & Haselton, Martie G. (2009). Vocal cues of ovulation in human females. Biology Letters, 5, 12–15.
Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12(1), 1–14.
Charbonnier, G. (1959). Entretiens avec Claude Levi-Strauss. https://www.jpbu.com/philo/.../Levi-Strauss_Charbonnier_Culture-langage.rtf.
Chomsky, A. N. (1975). Reflections on Language. New York: Pantheon Books.
Collins, S. A. (2000). Men’s voices and women’s choices. Animal Behaviour, 60(6), 773–780.
Collins, S. A., & Missing, C. (2003). Vocal and visual attractiveness are related in women. Animal Behaviour, 65(5), 997–1004.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. London, UK: John Murray.
Dunbar, R., Duncan, N., & Nettle, D. (1995). Size and structure of freely forming conversational groups. Human nature, 6(1), 67–78.
Evans, S., Neave, N., Wakelin, D., & Hamilton, C. (2008). The relationship between testosterone and vocal frequencies in human males. Physiology and Behavior, 93, 783–788.
Fairbanks, G. (1960). Voice and articulation. J. Cotler Books, 2nd revised edition.
Fant, G. (1960). Acoustic Theory of Speech Production. Berlin: De Gruyter.
Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Little, A. C. (2008a). Correlated preferences for men’s facial and vocal masculinity. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(4), 233–241.
Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Perrett, D. I. (2008b). The role of femininity and averageness of voice pitch in aesthetic judgments of women’s voices. Perception, 37(4), 615–623.
Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Moore, F. R., Law Smith, M. J., Cornwell, R. E., et al. (2005). The voice and face of woman: One ornament that signals quality? Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(5), 398–408.
Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., Law Smith, M. J., Moore, F. R., DeBruine, L. M., Cornwell, R. E., et al. (2006). Menstrual cycle, trait estrogen level, and masculinity preferences in the human voice. Hormones and Behavior, 49(2), 215–222.
Fischer, J., Semple, S., Fickenscher, G., Jürgens, R., Kruse, E., Heistermann, M., et al. (2011). Do women’s voices provide cues of the likelihood of ovulation? The importance of sampling regime. PLoS One, 6(9).
Fischer, A. H., & Manstead, A. S. (2000). The relation between gender and emotions in different cultures. Gender and emotion: Social psychological perspectives, 1, 71–94.
Fisher, M. L., & Voracek, M. (2006). The shape of beauty: determinants of female physical attractiveness. Journal of Cosmetic Dermatology, 5(2), 190–4.
Fitch, W. T., & Reby, D. (2001). The descended larynx is not uniquely human. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 268(1477), 1669–1675.
Fitch, W. T. (1997). Vocal tract length and format frequency dispersion correlated with body size in rhesus macaques. The Journal of the American Society of America, 102, 1213–1222.
Fitch, W. T., & Giedd, J. (1999). Morphology and development of the human vocal tract: A study using magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of the American Society of America, 106, 1511–1522.
Fraccaro, P. J., Jones, B. C., Vukovic, J., Smith, F. G., Watkins, C. D., Feinberg, D. R., et al. (2011). Experimental evidence that women speak in a higher voice pitch to men they find attractive. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 9(1), 57–67.
Fraccaro, P. J., O’Connor, J. J. M., Re, D. E., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., & Feinberg, D. R. (2013). Faking it: Deliberately altered voice pitch and vocal attractiveness. Animal Behaviour, 85(1), 127–136.
Geary, D. C., Vigil, J., & Byrd-Craven, J. (2004). Evolution of human mate choice. Journal of Sex Research, 41(1), 27–42.
Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845–857.
Handson, H. (1997). Glottal characteristics of female speakers: Acoustic correlates. The Journal of the American Society of America, 101(1), 466–81.
Hanson, H. M. (1997). Glottal characteristics of female speakers: Acoustic correlates. The Journal of the American Society of America, 101(1), 466–481.
Harris, A. C. (1994). Ethnicity as a determinant of sex role identity: A replication study of item selection for the Bem Sex Role Inventory. Sex Roles, 31, 241–273.
Haselton, M. G., Mortezaie, M., Pillsworth, E. G., Bleske-Rechek, A., & Frederick, D. A. (2007). Ovulatory shifts in human female ornamentation: Near ovulation, women dress to impress. Hormones and Behavior, 51, 40–45.
Higgins, M. B., & Saxman, J. H. (1989). Variations in vocal frequency perturbation across the menstrual cycle. Journal of Voice, 3, 233–243.
Hill, A. K., Hunt, J., Welling, L. L. M., Cárdenas, R. A., Rotella, M. A., Wheatley, J. R., et al. (2013). Quantifying the strength and form of sexual selection on men’s traits. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(5), 334–341.
Hodges-Simeon, C. R., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Puts, D. A. (2010). Different vocal parameters predict perceptions of dominance and attractiveness. Human Nature, 21(4), 406–427.
Holmberg, E. B., Hillman, R. E., Perkell, J. S., Guiod, P. C., & Goldman, S. L. (1995). Comparisons among aerodynamic, electroglottographic, andacoustic spectral measures of female voice. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 38, 1212–1223.
Hughes, S. M., Dispenza, F., & Gallup, G. G. (2004). Ratings of voice attractiveness predict sexual behavior and body configuration. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25, 295–304.
Hughes, S. M., Farley, S. D., & Rhodes, B. C. (2010). Vocal and physiological changes in response to the physical attractiveness of conversational partners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 34(3), 155–167.
Hughes, S. M., Mogilski, J. K., & Harrison, M. A. (2014). The perception and parameters of intentional voice manipulation. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 38(1), 107–127.
Hughes, S. M., Pastizzo, M. J., & Gallup, G. G. (2008). The sound of symmetry revisited: Subjective and objective analyses of voice. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32(2), 95–108.
Jones, B. C., Feinberg, D. R., DeBruine, L. M., Little, A. C., & Vukovic, J. (2010). A domain-specific opposite-sex bias in human preferences for manipulated voice pitch. Animal Behaviour, 79(1), 57–62.
Jones, B. C., Hahn, A. C., Fisher, C. I., Wang, H., Kandrik, M., Han, C., et al. (2018). No compelling evidence that preferences for facial masculinity track changes in women’s hormonal status. Psychological Science, 29(6), 10.
Jünger, J., Motta-Mena, N. V., Cardenas, R., Bailey, D., Rosenfield, K. A., Schild, C., et al. (2018). Do women’s preferences for masculine voices shift across the ovulatory cycle? Hormones and Behavior, 106, 122–134.
Kordsmeyer, T. L., Hunt, J., Puts, D. A., Ostner, J., & Penke, L. (2018). The relative importance of intra- and intersexual selection on human male sexually dimorphic traits. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(4), 424–436.
Landrine, H. (1985). Race x class stereotypes of women. Sex Roles, 13, 65–75.
Laver, J. (1980). The phonetic description of voice quality, (vol. 2009, 1st edn). Cambridge University Press.
Laver, J. (1980). The phonetic description of voice quality. Cambridge University Press.
Laver, J. (1994). Principles of phonetics. Cambridge University Press.
Leaderbrand, K., Dekam, J., Morey, A., & Tuma, L. (2008). The effects of voice pitch on perceptions of attractiveness: Do you sound hot or not? Winona State University Psychology Student Journal, 6.
Léon, P. (1981). BB ou la voix charmeuse, petite fille et coquette. Studia Phonetica, 18, 159–171.
Leongomez, J. D., Binter, J., Kubicova, L., Stolařova, P., Klapilova, K., Havlíček, J., et al. (2014). Vocal modulation during courtship increases proceptivity even in naive listeners. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(6), 489–496.
Lieberman, P. (1963). Some acoustic measures of the fundamental periodicity of normal and pathologic larynges. The Journal of the American Society of America, 35(3), 344–353.
Liu, X., & Xu, Y. (2011). What makes a female voice attractive? In Proceedings of ICPhS. Hong-Kong (pp. 1274–1277).
Marcinkowska, U. M., Galbarczyk, A., & Jasienska, G. (2018). La donna è mobile? Lack of cyclical shifts in facial symmetry, and facial and body masculinity preferences—A hormone based study. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 88, 47–53.
Miller, G. F., Tybur, J., & Jordan, B. (2007). Ovulatory cycle effects on tip earnings by lap-dancers: Economic evidence for human estrus? Evolution and Human Behavior, 6, 375–381.
Moore, P., & Thomson, C. L. (1965). Comments on physiology of hoarseness. Archives of Otolaryngology Head and Neck Surgery, 81(1), 97–102. https://doi.org/10.1001/archotol.1965.00750050102022.
Morton, E. S. (1977). On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. The America Naturalist, 111(981), 855–869.
Noble, W., & Davidson, I. (1996). Human evolution, language and mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
O’Connor, J. J. M., Fraccaro, P. J., Pisanski, K., Tigue, C. C., & Feinberg, D. R. (2013). Men’s preferences for women’s femininity in dynamic cross-modal stimuli. PLoS One, 8(7), e69531.
O’Connor, J. J., Re, D. E., & Feinberg, D. R. (2011). Voice pitch influences perceptions of sexual infidelity. Evolutionary Psychology, 9(1), 147470491100900109.
Oguchi, T., & Kikuchi, H. (1997). Voice and interpersonal attraction. Japanese Psychological Research, 39(1), 56–61.
Ohala, J. J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross- language utilisation of F0 of voice. Phonetica, 41(1), 1–16.
Pipitone, R. N., & Gallup, G. G. (2008). Women’s voice attractiveness varies across the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 29(4), 268–274.
Pisanski, K., Bhardwaj, K., & Reby, D. (2018). Women’s voice pitch lowers after pregnancy. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39(4), 457–463.
Pisanski, K., Fraccaro, P. J., Tigue, C. C., O’Connor, J. J. M., Röder, S., Andrews, P. W., et al. (2014). Vocal indicators of body size in men and women: A meta-analysis. Animal Behaviour, 95, 89–99.
Pisanski, K., & Rendall, D. (2011). The prioritization of voice fundamental frequency or formants in listeners’ assessments of speaker size, masculinity and attractiveness. The Journal of the American Society of America, 129(4), 2201–2212.
Polce-Lynch, M., Myers, B. J., Kilmartin, C. T., Forssmann-Falck, R., & Kliewer, W. (1998). Gender and age patterns in emotional expression, body image, and self-esteem: A qualitative analysis. Sex Roles, 38(11–12), 1025–1048.
Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(5), 388–397.
Puts, D. A. (2010). Beauty and the beast: Mechanisms of sexual selection in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(3), 157–175.
Puts, D. A., Bailey, D. H., Cárdenas, R. A., Burriss, R. P., Welling, L. L. M., Wheatley, J. R., et al. (2013). Women’s attractiveness changes with estradiol and progesterone across the ovulatory cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 63(1), 13–19.
Puts, D. A., Barndt, J. L., Welling, L. L. M., Dawood, K., & Burriss, R. P. (2011). Intrasexual competition among women: Vocal femininity affects perceptions of attractiveness and flirtatiousness. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(1), 111–115.
Puts, D. A., Gaulin, S. J. C., & Verdolini, K. (2006). Dominance and the evolution of sexual dimorphism in human voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(4), 283–296.
Renoult, J. P., Bovet, J., & Raymond, M. (2016). Beauty is in the efficient coding of the beholder. Royal Society Open Science, 3(3), 160027.
Re, D. E., O’Connor, J. J. M., Bennett, P. J., & Feinberg, D. R. (2012). Preferences for very low and very high voice pitch in humans. PLoS One, 7(3), e32719.
Revis, J. (2017). La voix et soi: Ce que notre voix dit de nous. France: Solal.
Riding, D., Lonsdale, D., & Brown, B. (2006). The effects of average fundamental frequency and variance of fundamental frequency on male vocal attractiveness to women. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 30(2), 55–61.
Saxton, T. K., Caryl, P. G., & Craig R. S. (2006). Vocal and facial attractiveness judgments of children, adolescents and adults: The ontogeny of mate choice. Ethology, 112(12), 1179–1185.
Saxton, T. K., Debruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., Little, A. C., & Roberts, S. C. (2009). Face and voice attractiveness judgments change during adolescence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(6), 398–408.
Sebesta, P., Kleisner, K., Turecek, P., Kočnar, T., Akoko, R. M., Trebicky, V., et al. (2017). Voices of Africa: Acoustic predictors of human male vocal attractiveness. Animal Behaviour, 127, 205–211.
Shirazi, T. N., Puts, D. A., & Escasa-Dorne, M. J. (2018). Filipino women’s preferences for male voice pitch: Intra-individual, life history, and hormonal predictors. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 4(2), 188–206.
Simmons, L. W., Peters, M., & Rhodes, G. (2011). Low pitched voices are perceived as masculine and attractive but do they predict semen quality in men? PLoS One, 6(12).
Singh, D. (1993). Adaptive significance of female physical attractiveness: Role of waist-to-hip ratio. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(2), 293–307.
Singh, D., & Young, R. K. (2001). Body weight, waist-to-hip ratio, breasts, and hips: Role in judgments of female attractiveness and desirability for relationships. Ethology and Sociobiology., 16(6), 483–507.
Skrinda, I., Krama, T., Kecko, S., Moore, F. R., Kaasik, A., Meija, L., et al. (2014). Body height, immunity, facial and vocal attractiveness in young men. Naturwissenschaften, 101(12), 1017–1025.
Södersten, M., Lindestad, P. A., & Hammarberg, B. (1991). Vocal fold closure, perceived breathiness, and acoustic characteristics in normal adult speakers. In J. Gauffin & B. Hammarberg (Eds.), Vocal fold physiology: Acoustic, perceptual, and physiological aspects of voice mechanisms (pp. 217–224).
Sughira, Y., & Katsurada, E. (1999). Masculinity and feminity in Japanese culture. Sex Roles, 40(718), 635–646.
Sugiyama, L. S. (2015). Physical attractiveness in adaptationist perspective. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 292–343). Wiley.
Suire, A., Raymond, M., & Barkat-Defradas, M. (2019). Male vocal quality and its relation to females’ preferences. Evolutionary Psychology, 1–12, https://doi.org/10.1177/1474704919874675.
Suire, A., Tognetti, A., Durand, V., Raymond, M., & Barkat-Defradas, M. (2020). The influence of sexual orientation and circulating testosterone levels on speech acoustic features. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 10, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-020-01665-3.
Suire, A., Raymond, M., & Barkat-Defradas, M. (2018). Vocal behavior within competitive and courtship contexts and its relation to mating success in humans. Evolution and Human Behavior, 39, 684–691.
Titze, I. R. (1994). Principles of voice production. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall.
Tognetti, A., Durand, V., Barkat-Defradas, M., & Hopfensitz, A. (2019). Does he sound cooperative? Acoustic correlates of cooperativeness. British Journal of Psychology, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12437.
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.
Traunmüller, H., & Eriksson, A. (1995). The frequency range of the voice fundamental in the speech of male and female adults. Unpublished manuscript.
Travers, L. M. (2017). Runaway selection. In J. Vonk & T. Shackelford (Eds.), Encyclopedia of animal cognition and behavior. Springer.
Tsantani, M. S., Belin, P., Paterson, H. M., & McAleer, P. (2016). Low vocal pitch preference drives first impressions irrespective of context in male voices but not in female voices. Perception, 45(8), 946–963.
Tuomi, S. K., & Fisher, J. E. (1979). Characteristics of simulated sexy voice. Folia Phoniatrica and Logopaedica, 31(4), 242–249.
Vaissière, J. (2015). La phonétique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.
van Bezooijen, R. (1995). Sociocultural aspects of pitch differences between Japanese and Dutch women. Language and Speech, 38(3), 253–265.
van Borsel, J., Janssens, J., & De Bodt, M. (2009). Breathiness as a feminine voice characteristic: A perceptual approach. Journal of Voice, 23(3), 291–294.
Vorperian, H. K., Kent, R. D., Lindstrom, M. J., Kalina, C. M., Gentry, L. R., & Yandell, B. S. (2005). Development of vocal tract length during early childhood: A magnetic resonance imaging study. The Journal of the American Society of America, 117, 338–350.
Vukovic, J., Feinberg, D. R., Jones, B. C., DeBruine, L. M., Welling, L. L. M., Little, A. C., et al. (2008). Self-rated attractiveness predicts individual differences in women’s preferences for masculine men’s voices. Personality and Individual Differences, 45(6), 451–456.
Xu, Y., Lee, A., Wu, W.-L., Liu, X., & Birkholz, P. (2013). Human vocal attractiveness as signaled by body size projection. PLoS One, 8(4), e62397.
Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection—A selection for handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.
Zheng, Y., Compton, B. J., Heyman, G. D., & Jiang, Z. (2020). Vocal attractiveness and voluntarily pitch-shifted voices. Evolution and Human Behavior.
Zuckerman, M., Driver, R. E. (1989). What sounds beautiful is good: The vocal attractiveness stereotype. Journal of nonverbal behavior, 13, 67–82, https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00990791
Zuckerman, M., & Miyake, K. (1993). The attractive voice: What makes it so? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 17(2), 119–135.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Editor information
Editors and Affiliations
Rights and permissions
Copyright information
© 2021 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.
About this chapter
Cite this chapter
Barkat-Defradas, M., Raymond, M., Suire, A. (2021). Vocal Preferences in Humans: A Systematic Review. In: Weiss, B., Trouvain, J., Barkat-Defradas, M., Ohala, J.J. (eds) Voice Attractiveness. Prosody, Phonology and Phonetics. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-6627-1_4
Download citation
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-6627-1_4
Published:
Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore
Print ISBN: 978-981-15-6626-4
Online ISBN: 978-981-15-6627-1
eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)