Skip to main content

Comparing Chomsky, Skinner and Harris: What Counts as ‘New’?

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Models of the Human in Twentieth-Century Linguistic Theories
  • 287 Accesses

Abstract

In this comparison concerning the second topic ‘what counts as “new” ’, an interesting discovery emerging from our discussion is that the authors’ differences in conceptualizing the ‘new’ are fundamentally influenced by what is considered ‘constant’. Chomsky believes in an innate organ of grammar which can be captured through scientific linguistic research. This linguistic organ generates all and only grammatical sentences in a specific language. The creativity exemplified by speakers and hearers in producing and understanding new sentences can be directly derived from the functioning of this organ. Skinner, on the other hand, believes that human behaviour is actually continuous and undifferentiated though, as observed in adults, it features a diversified repertoire resulting from emergence of new forms of behaviours. Not only that, he also explains how the shaping of new behaviour depends upon the recognition that human behaviour is in nature continuous. While Harris emphasizes the ever-newness of meaning generated in the flow of contexts, he also implies the sameness of all integration process only that this process is guided by the principle of cotemporality and is time-bound. Moreover, the three authors’ accounts reflect three different views of ‘temporality’. To put in general terms, Chomsky’s theory abstracts away ‘time’ by not looking into language use; so what is new is virtually already there. Skinner does not recognize the time-boundedness of human behaviour; instead, he believes in the existence of general laws of behaviour which could be applied repetitively to produce and control behaviours. In contrast, Harris strongly underlines the situatedness of human communication and sign-making; his understanding of ‘new’ meaning recognizes the fundamental shiftiness of time and contexts.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The corresponding analysis of functional relations specific to each kind of verbal behavior can be found in Skinner (1957, pp. 185–186).

  2. 2.

    The dependent variable is defined as ‘the probability that a verbal response of given form will occur at a given time’ (Skinner, 1957, p. 28) and it is the basic datum to be predicted and controlled. Independent variables are in turn the conditions and events to be changed in order to achieve prediction or control. They include: controlling stimuli, reinforcement, deprivation, aversive stimulation, certain emotional conditions (Skinner, 1957, p. 199). Skinner further develops a concept called ‘operant’ which is one form of response ‘concerned with the prediction and control of a kind of behaviour’ (Skinner, 1957, p. 20).

  3. 3.

    Refer to the following quotation in Skinner (1965, p. 108): ‘we describe the contingency by saying that a stimulus (the light) is the occasion upon which a response (stretching the neck) is followed by reinforcement (with food). The effect upon the pigeon is that eventually the response is more likely to occur when the light is on. The process through which this comes about is called discrimination.’

References

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton & Co..

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Culy, C. (1998). Statistical distribution and the grammatical/ungrammatical distinction. Grammars, 1(1), 1–13.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harris, R. (1998). Introduction to integrational linguistics. London: Pergamon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, R. (2004). Integrationism, language, mind and world. Language Sciences, 26(6), 727–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutton, C. (2011). The politics of the language myth: Reflections on the writings of Roy Harris. Language Sciences, 33(4), 503–510.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, J. (1991). Chomsky. London: Fontana Press. (Original work published 1970).

    Google Scholar 

  • Sampson, G. (2001). Empirical linguistics. London: Continuum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1965). Science and human behavior. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Verhaar, J. W. M. (1973). Phenomenology and present-day linguistics. In M. Natanson (Ed.), Phenomenology and the social sciences (Vol. 1, pp. 361–451). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Zhou, F. (2020). Comparing Chomsky, Skinner and Harris: What Counts as ‘New’?. In: Models of the Human in Twentieth-Century Linguistic Theories. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-15-1255-1_19

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics