Skip to main content

Sensory Connections and Emotional Knowledge

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Making Sense of ‘Food’ Animals
  • 292 Accesses

Abstract

Turning to sensory and emotional associations with meat and food animals, this chapter begins by situating this part of the book in the broader literature on senses, emotions, and affect. It then demonstrates how participants’ senses inform their determinations of ‘good’ and ‘bad’ meat, conceiving the senses as the link between Foucault’s knowing and pleasure. This leads to the main focus of the chapter, which is how the emotions expressed and identified by participants contribute to an embodied mapping, or ‘making sense’ of ‘food’ animals and ‘meat’. Noting how particular emotions, associated with comfort and discomfort, become associated with different animals and meat, the chapter examines where and how an embodied knowledge of ‘food’ animals is challenged and how their edibility is maintained.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Acknowledging these are the senses most associated with human experience and that other animals may have many others.

  2. 2.

    Elisabeth Hsu (2008: 436–437) notes that ‘sensory experience’, rather than ‘sensory perception’, is often used to avoid the traditional conception of the mind-matter dualism that originated in the natural scientific framework.

  3. 3.

    Importantly for qualitative research, Beekman highlights that the reverse is also true—that there is also knowing in pleasure, and argues for using emotional perception as a source of knowledge about food by “listen[ing] to the aesthetic or cultural knowledge embedded in people’s emotional responses” (2006: 309).

  4. 4.

    For example, Lim (2016); Mesquita and Frijda (1992) and De Leersnyder et al. (2015), to name a few.

  5. 5.

    In relation to my third objective, I conceive emotional discomfort to be indicative of challenges to an embodied knowledge of animals’ normalised edibility. My explorations of emotional associations are thus framed in terms of comfort, which maintains, and discomfort, which challenges this edibility. As well as avoiding the binary perspective of positive and negative emotions, these terms reflect the terminology used most frequently by my participants. I conceive them as my empirically supported variation on Foucault’s conception of ‘pleasure’.

  6. 6.

    Also known as cultured meat, Miller defines in vitro meat as “meat that is grown by proliferating cells in a nutrient-rich medium without the necessity of an animal’s slaughter” (2012: 42). Some production methods rely on ‘donor’ animals and animal products, notably calf serum, although animal-free media are starting to be used. Much debate and contention surrounds this developing technological field (Pluhar 2009; Stephens 2010; Chiles 2013), and for many, the perpetuation of a ‘carniculture’, where ‘meat’, even animal-free meat, is prioritised “remains in need of careful thought” (Miller 2012: 43).

  7. 7.

    I conceive this comfort as associated not only with other meaty practices, or elements thereof, but also with, and constituted by, the worldwide nexus of practices—environmental, social, economic, medical, educational, and so on—by which meat consumption and the use of ‘food’ animals have been universally, and systemically, normalised. This aggregate comfort is set against, and enhanced by, the discomfort—the “unpleasant negative feelings, a state of tension” (Williams and Irurita 2006: 408)—associated with plant-based dietary practices situated within or alongside practices where meat has been normalised. Admittedly, at the time of writing, this discomfort is being unsettled to a greater degree in Westernised nations than at any other time in recent history.

  8. 8.

    One participant informed me six months after our interview that she no longer ate meat. Having not followed up with the rest of my participants, I cannot say with any authority that they all continue to eat meat, or even that this one participant continues to not eat meat.

  9. 9.

    I acknowledge here that the production and/or consumption of ethical and sustainable meat were my main recruitment criteria and therefore this is not surprising.

  10. 10.

    As a teenager, Joyce spent two years following a vegetarian diet and 6–12 months following a vegan diet. With respect to her current practices, she explains, “my body seems to be a body that needs to eat meat”.

  11. 11.

    I do not count reducing meat consumption as defection because it still involves the persistence of practices relating to eating animals and their constitution as food.

  12. 12.

    The findings of this US study, involving 11,429 people over 17 years, have recently been critiqued by Lockwood (2019), specifically the aggregation of vegetarians and vegans (veg∗ns) into one group, the question of whether dietary veganism is a ‘plant-based diet’ as opposed to veganism (i.e. an ethical commitment to oppose all animal exploitation), the relatively small sample size, and the underlying research design. A much larger UK study, conducted by EPIC-Oxford between the 1990s and 2010 with approximately 65,000 people over 35, found much smaller rates of recidivism over time (15%–27%). Further questions could be posed of both studies regarding the reliability of data from self-reporting participants, the risk of virtue signalling, and the contribution of age and ageing populations to the findings (Massow et al. 2019), all of which point to a need for more qualitative research to explore understandings and everyday practices surrounding both dietary and ethical veganism.

  13. 13.

    This point relates to a large body of literature on social conformity.

References

  • ACE. (2017). Length of Adherence to Vegetarianism. Animal Charity Evaluators. Online. Retrieved April 2018, from https://animalcharityevaluators.org/research/dietary-impacts/vegetarian-recidivism/.

  • Adams, C. J. (2010). The Sexual Politics of Meat: A Feminist-vegetarian Critical Theory. London; New York: Continuum International Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahmed, S. (2004a). Affective Economies. Social Text, 22(2), 117–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ahmed, S. (2004b). The Cultural Politics of Emotion. New York; London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ahmed, S. (2010). The Promise of Happiness. Durham; London: Duke University Press Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, E. C., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). Affective Beliefs Influence the Experience of Eating Meat. PLoS One, 11(8), 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Appadurai, A. (1996). Modernity at Large: Cultural Dimensions of Globalization. Minneapolis; London: University of Minnesota Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Asher, K., Green, C., Gutbrod, H., Jewell, M., et al. (2014). Study of Current and Former Vegetarians and Vegans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, B., & Loughnan, S. (2016). Resolving the Meat-Paradox: A Motivational Account of Morally Troublesome Behavior and Its Maintenance. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 21(3), 278–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bastian, B., Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., & Radke, H. R. M. (2012). Don’t Mind Meat? The Denial of Mind to Animals Used for Human Consumption. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(2), 247–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beedie, P., & Hudson, S. (2003). Emergence of Mountain-Based Adventure Tourism. Annals of Tourism Research, 30(3), 625–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beekman, V. (2006). Feeling Food: The Rationality of Perception. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 19(3), 301–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bittman, M. (2013). VB6: Eat Vegan Before 6:00 to Lose Weight and Restore Your Health … for Good. New York: Hachette Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Boo, J. (2016, May 19). How Many Vegans? One of the Fastest Growing Lifestyle Movements. The Huffington Post. Online. October 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brymer, E., & Schweitzer, R. (2013). Extreme Sports Are Good for Your Health: A Phenomenological Understanding of Fear and Anxiety in Extreme Sport. Journal of Health Psychology, 18(4), 477–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnett, C. (2001). Whose Game Is It Anyway? Power, Play and Sport. Agenda, 16(49), 71–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, S. (1994). Being Dismissed: The Politics of Emotional Expression. Hypatia, 9(3), 46–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carnicelli-Filho, S., Schwartz, G. M., & Tahara, A. K. (2010). Fear and Adventure Tourism in Brazil. Tourism Management, 31(6), 953–956.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, E. Y., & Zlatevska, N. (2019). Jerkies, Tacos, and Burgers: Subjective Socioeconomic Status and Meat Preference. Appetite, 132(1), 257–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chau, A. Y. (2008). The Sensorial Production of the Social. Ethnos, 73(4), 485–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiles, R. M. (2013). Intertwined Ambiguities: Meat, in Vitro Meat, and the Ideological Construction of the Marketplace. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 12(6), 472–482.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Citrin, L. B., Roberts, T.-A., & Fredrickson, B. L. (2004). Objectification Theory and Emotions: A Feminist Psychological Perspective on Gendered Affect. In L. Z. Tiedens & C. W. Leach (Eds.), The Social Life of Emotions (pp. 203–226). Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (2011). From “Animal Machines” to “Happy Meat”? Foucault’s Ideas of Disciplinary and Pastoral Power Applied to ‘Animal-Centred’ Welfare Discourse. Animals, 1(4), 83–101.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, E. (2015, March 17). Vegan Is Going Mainstream, Trend Data Suggests. Food navigator.com . October 2016.

  • Dalziell, J., & Wadiwel, D. J. (2016). Live Exports, Animal Advocacy, Race and ‘Animal Nationalism’. In A. Potts (Ed.), Meat Culture (pp. 73–89). Leiden; Boston: BRILL.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Leersnyder, J., Boiger, M., & Mesquita, B. (2015). Cultural Differences in Emotions. In An Interdisciplinary, Searchable, and Linkable Resource (pp. 1–15). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devi, M. (1997). Rudali, from Fiction to Performance. Kolkata: Seagull Book Pvt. Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, A. B., & Miele, M. (2012). Between Food and Flesh: How Animals Are Made to Matter (and Not Matter) within Food Consumption Practices. Environment and Planning D: Society and Space, 30(2), 298–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flower, S. (2018). The Part-time Vegan. Hachette Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1978). The History of Sexuality. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1982). The Subject and Power. Critical Inquiry, 8(4), 777–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foucault, M. (1989). The Order of Things. London; New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goud, N. H. (2005). Courage: Its Nature and Development. The Journal of Humanistic Counseling, 44(1), 102–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Conroy, A., & Hayes-Conroy, J. (2008). Taking Back Taste: Feminism, Food and Visceral Politics. Gender, Place & Culture, 15(5), 461–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes-Conroy, A., & Hayes-Conroy, J. (2010). Visceral Difference: Variations in Feeling (Slow) Food. Environment and Planning A, 42(12), 2956–2971.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hollows, J. (2003). Oliver’s Twist. International Journal of Cultural Studies, 6(2), 229–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hsu, E. (2008). The Senses and the Social: An Introduction. Ethnos, 73(4), 433–443.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hurlstone, L. D. (2011). Performing Marginal Identities: Understanding the Cultural Significance of Tawa’if and Rudali Rough the Language of the Body in South Asian Cinema. Dissertation and Thesis. Paper 154. Master of Science in Communication. Portland State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Illouz, E. (2009). Emotions, Imagination and Consumption: A New Research Agenda. Journal of Consumer Culture, 9(3), 377–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T. D. (1978). A Social Interactional Theory of Emotions. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinsella, E. L., Ritchie, T. D., & Igou, E. R. (2017). On the Bravery and Courage of Heroes: Considering Gender. Heroism Science, 2(1), 1–14.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirkwood, K. (2016, May 26). Dude Food vs Superfood: We’re Cultural Omnivores. The Conversation. Online. October 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Knight, D. (1999). Why We Enjoy Condemning Sentimentality: A Meta-aesthetic Perspective. The Journal of Aesthetics and Art Criticism, 57(4), 411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kolcaba, K. (2003). Comfort Theory and Practice: A Vision for Holistic Health Care and Research. New York: Springer Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korsmeyer, C. (2002). Making Sense of Taste. New York: Cornell University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levi-Strauss, C. (1991). Totemism. London: Merlin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, P., & Simpson, R. (2007). Gendering Emotions in Organizations. Basingstoke; New York: Palgrave.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lim, N. (2016). Cultural Differences in Emotion: Differences in Emotional Arousal Level between the East and the West. Integrative Medicine Research, 5(2), 105–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Locher, J. L., Yoels, W. C., Maurer, D., & van Ells, J. (2005). Comfort Foods: An Exploratory Journey into the Social and Emotional Significance of Food. Food and Foodways, 13(4), 273–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockwood, A. (2019, January 7). Do 84% of Vegans and Vegetarians Really Go Back to Eating Meat? Plant Based News. Online. April 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  • Loughnan, S., Bastian, B., & Haslam, N. (2014). The Psychology of Eating Animals. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 104–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lupton, D. (1996). Food, the Body and the Self. London; Thousand Oaks; New Delhi: SAGE Publications Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • von Massow, M., Weersink, A., & Gallant, M. (2019, March 12). Meat Consumption Is Changing But It’s Not Because of Vegans. The Conversation. Online. April 2019.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menely, T. (2007). Zoophilpsychosis: Why Animals Are What’s Wrong with Sentimentality. Symploke, 15(1/2), 244–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. H. (1992). Cultural Variations in Emotions: A Review. Psychol Bulletin, 112(2), 179–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mesquita, B., & Walker, R. (2003). Cultural Differences in Emotions: A Context for Interpreting Emotional Experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 41(7), 777–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. (2012). In Vitro Meat: Power, Authenticity and Vegetarianism. Journal for Critical Animal Studies, 10(4), 41–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molz, J. G. (2007). Eating Difference: The Cosmopolitan Mobilities of Culinary Tourism. Space and Culture, 10(1), 77–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mykletun, R. J., & GyimOthy, S. (2010). Beyond the Renaissance of the Traditional Voss Sheep’s-Head Meal: Tradition, Culinary Art, Scariness and Entrepreneurship. Tourism Management, 31(3), 434–446.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Narula, S.K. (2014, April 9). What’s Wrong with Sentimentality? The Atlantic. Online. January 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parry, J. (2010). Gender and Slaughter in Popular Gastronomy. Feminism & Psychology, 20(3), 381–396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piqueras-Fiszman, B., & Spence, C. (2015). Sensory Expectations Based on Product-Extrinsic Food Cues: An Interdisciplinary Review of the Empirical Evidence and Theoretical Accounts. Food Quality and Preference, 40, 165–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pluhar, E. B. (2009). Meat and Morality: Alternatives to Factory Farming. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics, 23(5), 455–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Probyn, E. (2000). Carnal Appetites: FoodSexIdentities. London; New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reckwitz, A. (2002). Toward a Theory of Social Practices: A Development in Culturalist Theorizing. European Journal of Social Theory, 5(2), 243–263.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reckwitz, A. (2016). Practices and Their Affects. In A. Hui, T. Schatzki, & E. Shove (Eds.), The Nexus of Practices: Connections, Constellations, Practitioners (pp. 114–125). London; New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, B. (2013). The Ethical Butcher: How Thoughtful Eating Can Change Your World. Berkeley, CA: Soft Skull Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruby, M. B., & Heine, S. J. (2011). Meat, Morals, and Masculinity. Appetite, 56(2), 447–450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seltzer, L. F. (2015, October 21). The Complex Emotion of Courage: Do You Really Understand It? Psychology Today. Online. October 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shove, E. (2003). Converging Conventions of Comfort, Cleanliness and Convenience. Journal of Consumer Policy, 26(4), 395–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, H. (2016). Writing the Fleischgeist. Animal Studies Journal, 5(2), 183–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singh, J., Khanna, A., & Khanna, P. K. (2014). Rudali’ as an Epitome of Caste, Class and Gender Subalternity: An Analysis of Mahasweta Devi’s Rudali. Indian Journal of Applied Research, 4(7), 282–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sobal, J. (2005). Men, Meat, and Marriage: Models of Masculinity. Food and Foodways, 13(102), 135–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, R. C. (2004). In Defence of Sentimentality. New York; Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stallen, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2015). The Neuroscience of Social Conformity: Implications for Fundamental and Applied Research. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 9, 337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, N. (2010). In Vitro Meat: Zombies on the Menu? SCRIPTed, 7(2), 394–401.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stone, P., & Sharpley, R. (2008). Consuming Dark Tourism: A Thanatological Perspective. Annals of Tourism Research, 35(2), 574–595.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, D. E. (2010). Food and the Senses. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39, 209–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tarlow, P. E. (2007). Dark Tourism: The Appealing ‘Dark’ Side of Tourism and More. In M. Novelli (Ed.), Niche Tourism: Contemporary Issues, Trends and Cases (pp. 47–58). London; New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. H., & Stets, J. E. (2005). The Sociology of Emotions. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. H., & Stets, J. E. (2006). Sociological Theories of Human Emotions. Annual Review of Sociology, 32(1), 25–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Twine, R. (2014). Vegan Killjoys at the Table—Contesting Happiness and Negotiating Relationships with Food Practices. Societies, 4, 623–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vannini, P., Waskul, D., & Gottschalk, S. (2013). The Senses in Self, Society, and Culture: A Sociology of the Senses. New York; London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Walker, K. (2016, June 17). Become a Part-Time Vegan and Get Healthy. Body and Soul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wansink, B., Cheney, M. M., & Chan, N. (2003). Exploring Comfort Food Preferences Across Age and Gender. Physiology & Behavior, 79(4–5), 739–747.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wetherell, M. (2012). Affect and Emotion: A New Social Science Understanding. London; Thousand Oaks; New Delhi: SAGE Publications Ltd.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M., & Irurita, V. F. (2006). Emotional Comfort: The Patient’s Perspective of a Therapeutic Context. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 43(4), 405–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, A. M., Lester, L., Bulsara, C., Petterson, A., et al. (2017). Patient Evaluation of Emotional Comfort Experienced (PEECE): Developing and Testing a Measurement Instrument. BMJ Open, 7, e012999.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paula Arcari .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 The Author(s)

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Arcari, P. (2020). Sensory Connections and Emotional Knowledge. In: Making Sense of ‘Food’ Animals. Palgrave Macmillan, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-9585-7_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-9585-7_6

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Palgrave Macmillan, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-13-9584-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-13-9585-7

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics