Skip to main content

Segmentation in Translation: A Look at Expert Behaviour

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Researching Cognitive Processes of Translation

Part of the book series: New Frontiers in Translation Studies ((NFTS))

Abstract

The present contribution examines an interesting but technically quite imperfect sample recording in the CRITT Centre’s TPR-database in an attempt to demonstrate how imperfect gaze data can be meaningfully reconstructed and to illustrate and explore details of translational keystroke and gaze behaviour in a single translator. The data clearly show that translation proceeds segment by segment. A source text (ST) segment is read, comprehended, and translated. As the translation is typed, we see it emerging segment by segment. Perfectly smooth production of target text across extended stretches of time is not frequently seen, but is often approximated. Highly expert performers are able to bind processing segments together into a flow of continuous production. From their recorded gaze behaviour, we can observe that experts do process text segment by segment, so how is it that they can manage to sometimes maintain fairly continuous production? Evidence of how reading, comprehension, translation, formulation and typing activities are coordinated is found in recorded gaze data, which provide detailed evidence of what ST text unit was being worked on at any given point in time, and evidence provided by keystrokes. These combined sources of evidence can be used to infer both what ST (sub)segment was being processed within what ST context, and in what manner, always with the big unknowns at play of the translator’s knowledge, memory, meaning construction intelligence and expressive power – and the suspicion that the human brain is doing a good deal more than eye movements and keystrokes reveal.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 69.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 89.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 119.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Gile’s ‘tightrope’ metaphor, which aimed at describing how a simultaneous interpreter operates close to cognitive saturation, having to balance several cognitive efforts at the same time: listening, reformulating, speaking (Gile 1999), could be relevantly considered in relation to expert and optimally fluent translation performance. The data presented below seems to clearly indicate that a translator’s attention may also be split or have to travel extremely fast between reading, reformulating, typing and monitoring to a point where one wonders if such performance is possible with only 7 +/− 2 items of STM capacity available, according to the famous observation by Miller (1956, p. 81).

  2. 2.

    Translog II from 2011 was designed by professor Michael Carl. It was based on the code of the experimental Translog 2006 version designed by the author, programmed by his son Lasse Schou, and used in the EU Eye-to-IT project (2006–2009). Translog II can be freely downloaded at https://sites.google.com/site/centretranslationinnovation/translog-ii

  3. 3.

    http://www.inputlog.net/

  4. 4.

    www.translog.dk. This version is no longer distributed and the site will be closed down in 2018.

  5. 5.

    Keystroke intervals have been calculated from the raw data in the xml file.

  6. 6.

    Segments 16 and 17 have been comprehensively analysed in Jakobsen (2016).

  7. 7.

    Translog II also has a GWM (gaze-to-word mapping) function designed to automatically identify words that are fixated. GWM was not used in the present study.

  8. 8.

    A translation process model could include several modalities, listening, reading and watching at the input end, speaking, writing, signing at the production end.

  9. 9.

    As will be seen in subsequent figures, there is also vertical displacement in the representation of gaze samples and fixations in this recording. Fixations on the headline are well aligned vertically, but the fixations on the second line mostly appear above the line, and the same applies to fixations on lines three and four. Towards the bottom end of the screen we see the reverse phenomenon that the lower on the screen fixations are displayed, the more they tend to be placed lower than their probable target. Inaccurate calibration can also explain why left-eye samples (in red) often appear lower down on the screen than (green) right-eye samples, except in the top and bottom right areas.

  10. 10.

    The interested reader is invited to download a free copy of Translog II and access and replay the P01_T1 file from the SG12 experiment, which is also freely accessible at:https://sites.google.com/site/centretranslationinnovation/

  11. 11.

    A speculation might be that the translator was considering a more technical medical translation of ‘amounts’.

  12. 12.

    Schaeffer (2013) makes an interesting case that literal, word-to-word translation is faster and cognitively easier than translation which involves rearrangement of word order.

References

  • Angelone, E. (2010). Uncertainty, uncertainty management and metacognitive problem solving in the translation task. In G. M. Shreve & E. Angelone (Eds.), Translation and cognition (pp. 17–40). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayer-Hohenwarter, G. (2012). Translatorische kreativität: Definition-messung-entwicklung. Tübingen: Narr Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butterworth, B. (1980). Evidence from pauses in speech. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language production. Volume 1. Speech and talk (pp. 155–176). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carl, M., & Dragsted, B. (2012). Inside the monitor model: Processes of default and challenged translation production. Translation: Computation, Corpora, Cognition. Special Issue on the Crossroads Between Contrastive Linguistics, Translation Studies and Machine Translation, 2(1), 127–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carl, M., & Kay, M. (2011). Gazing and typing activities during translation: A comparative study of translation units of professional and student translators. Meta, 56(4), 952–975.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dragsted, B. (2004). Segmentation in translation and translation memory systems: An empirical investigation of cognitive segmentation and effects of integrating a TM system into the translation process. PhD thesis. Copenhagen Business School, Frederiksberg, Denmark.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dragsted, B. (2010). Coordination of reading and writing processes in translation: An eye on uncharted territory. In G. M. Shreve & E. Angelone (Eds.), Translation and cognition (pp. 41–62). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dragsted, B., & Hansen, I. G. (2008). Comprehension and production in translation: A pilot study on segmentation and the coordination of reading and writing processes. In S. Göpferich, A. L. Jakobsen, & I. M. Mees (Eds.), Looking at eyes: Eyetracking studies of reading and translation processing (pp. 9–29). Copenhagen: Samfundslitteratur.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gile, D. (1999). Testing the effort models’ tightrope hypothesis in simultaneous interpreting – A contribution. Hermes, 23, 153–172.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldman-Eisler, F. (1972). Pauses, clauses, sentences. Language and Speech, 15(2), 103–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Immonen, S., & Mäkisalo, J. (2010). Pauses reflecting the processing of syntactic units in monolingual text production and translation. Hermes, 44, 45–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ivir, V. (1981). Formal correspondence vs. translation equivalence revisited. Poetics Today, 2(4), 51–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakobsen, A. L. (2002). Translation drafting by professional translators and by translation students. Traducción & Comunicación, 3, 89–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakobsen, A. L. (2003). Effects of think aloud on translation speed, revision, and segmentation. In F. Alves (Ed.), Triangulating translation: Perspectives in process oriented research (pp. 69–95). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jakobsen, A. L. (2005). Instances of peak performance in translation. Lebende Sprachen, 50(3), 111–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakobsen, A. L. (2016). Are Gaze Shifts a Key to a Translator’s Text Segmentation? Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics, 52(2), 149–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schaeffer, M. (2013). The ideal literal translation hypothesis: The role of shared representations during translation. PhD thesis. University of Leicester, Leicester.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schilperoord, J. (1996). It’s about time: Temporal aspects of cognitive processes in text production. Amsterdam: Rodopi.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toury, G. (2012). Descriptive translation studies – And beyond (Rev. Ed.). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Arnt Lykke Jakobsen .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2019 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Jakobsen, A.L. (2019). Segmentation in Translation: A Look at Expert Behaviour. In: Li, D., Lei, V., He, Y. (eds) Researching Cognitive Processes of Translation. New Frontiers in Translation Studies. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-1984-6_4

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-13-1984-6_4

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-13-1983-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-13-1984-6

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics