Advertisement

Predicting Readers’ Sarcasm Understandability by Modeling Gaze Behavior

  • Abhijit MishraEmail author
  • Pushpak Bhattacharyya
Chapter
  • 671 Downloads
Part of the Cognitive Intelligence and Robotics book series (CIR)

Abstract

In the previous two chapters, we demonstrated how cognitive effort in text annotation can be assessed by utilizing cognitive information obtained from readers’/annotators’ eye-gaze patterns. While our models are, to some extent, effective in modeling various forms of complexities at the textual side, we observed that cognitive information can also be useful to model the ability of a reader to understand/comprehend the given reading material. This observation was quite clear in our sentiment annotation experiment (discussed in Chap. 3), where the eye-movement patterns of some of our annotators appeared to be subtle when the text had linguistic nuances like sarcasm, which the annotators failed to recognize. This motivated us to work on a highly specific yet important problem of sarcasm understandability prediction—a starting step toward an even more important problem of modeling text comprehensibility.

Keywords

Modeling Gaze Behavior Text Side Linguistic Nuances Scanpath Understanding Irony 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Barbieri, F., Saggion, H., & Ronzano, F. (2014). Modelling sarcasm in Twitter, a novel approach. In 2014, ACL (p. 50).Google Scholar
  2. Camblin, C. C., Gordon, P. C., & Swaab, T. Y., et al. (2007). The interplay of discourse congruence and lexical association during sentence processing: Evidence from ERPs and eye tracking. Journal of Memory and Language, 56(1), 103–128.Google Scholar
  3. Campbell, J. D., & Katz, A. N. (2012). Are there necessary conditions for inducing a sense of sarcastic irony? Discourse Processes, 49(6), 459–480.Google Scholar
  4. Carvalho, P., Sarmento, L., Silva, M. J., & De Oliveira, E. (2009). Clues for detecting irony in user-generated contents: oh...!! it’s so easy;-). In Proceedings of the 1st International CIKM Workshop on Topic-Sentiment Analysis for Mass Opinion (pp. 53–56). ACM.Google Scholar
  5. Clark, H. H., & Gerrig, R. J. (1984). On the pretense theory of irony. 113(1), 121.Google Scholar
  6. Davidov, D., Tsur, O., & Rappoport, A. (2010). Semi-supervised recognition of sarcastic sentences in Twitter and Amazon. In Proceedings of the Fourteenth Conference on Computational Natural Language Learning (pp. 107–116). Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
  7. Filik, R., Leuthold, H., Wallington, K., & Page, J. (2014). Testing theories of irony processing using eye-tracking and ERPS. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 811–828.Google Scholar
  8. Gibbs, R. W. (1986). Comprehension and memory for nonliteral utterances: The problem of sarcastic indirect requests. Acta Psychologica, 62(1), 41–57.Google Scholar
  9. Giora, R. (1995). On irony and negation. Discourse Processes, 19(2), 239–264.Google Scholar
  10. González-Ibánez, R., Muresan, S., & Wacholder, N. (2011). Identifying sarcasm in Twitter: A closer look. In Proceedings of the 49th Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics: Human Language Technologies: Short Papers (Vol. 2, pp. 581–586). Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
  11. Hall, M., Frank, E., Holmes, G., Pfahringer, B., Reutemann, P., & Witten, I. H. (2009). The weka data mining software: An update. ACM SIGKDD Explorations Newsletter, 11(1), 10–18.Google Scholar
  12. Holmqvist, K., Nyström, M., Andersson, R., Dewhurst, R., Jarodzka, H., & Van de Weijer, J. (2011). Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Ivanko, S. L., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Context incongruity and irony processing. Discourse Processes, 35(3), 241–279.Google Scholar
  14. Jorgensen, J., Miller, G. A., & Sperber, D. (1984). Test of the mention theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(1), 112.Google Scholar
  15. Joshi, A., Sharma, V., & Bhattacharyya, P. (2015). Harnessing context incongruity for sarcasm detection. In Proceedings of 53rd Annual Meeting of the Association for Computational Linguistics, Beijing, China (p. 757).Google Scholar
  16. Kincaid, J. P., Fishburne, R. P. Jr., Rogers, R. L., & Chissom, B. S. (1975). Derivation of new readability formulas (automated readability index, fog count and flesch reading ease formula) for navy enlisted personnel. Technical report, DTIC Document.Google Scholar
  17. Kutas, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1980). Reading senseless sentences: Brain potentials reflect semantic incongruity. Science, 207(4427), 203–205.Google Scholar
  18. Liebrecht, C., Kunneman, F., & van den Bosch, A. (2013). The perfect solution for detecting sarcasm in tweets# not. In2013, WASSA (p.  29).Google Scholar
  19. Martınez-Gómez, P., & Aizawa, A. (2013). Diagnosing causes of reading difficulty using Bayesian networks. In 2013, IJCNLP.Google Scholar
  20. Maynard, D. & Greenwood, M. A. (2014). Who cares about sarcastic tweets? investigating the impact of sarcasm on sentiment analysis. In Proceedings of LREC.Google Scholar
  21. Pang, B., & Lee, L. (2004). A sentimental education: Sentiment analysis using subjectivity summarization based on minimum cuts. In Proceedings of the 42nd Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics (p. 271). Association for Computational Linguistics.Google Scholar
  22. Parasuraman, R., & Rizzo, M. (2006). Neuroergonomics: The brain at work. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  23. Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin, 124(3), 372.Google Scholar
  24. Riloff, E., Qadir, A., Surve, P., De Silva, L., Gilbert, N., & Huang, R. (2013). Sarcasm as contrast between a positive sentiment and negative situation. In Proceedings of Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (pp. 704–714).Google Scholar
  25. Shamay, S., Tomer, R., & Aharon, J. (2005). The neuroanatomical basis of understanding sarcasm and its relationship to social cognition. Neuropsychology, 19(3), 288.Google Scholar
  26. Von der Malsburg, T., & Vasishth, S. (2011). What is the scanpath signature of syntactic reanalysis? Journal of Memory and Language, 65(2), 109–127.Google Scholar
  27. Xu, X., & Frank, E. (2004). Logistic regression and boosting for labeled bags of instances. In Advances in knowledge discovery and data mining (pp. 272–281). Springer.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd. 2018

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.India Research LabIBM ResearchBangaloreIndia
  2. 2.Indian Institute of Technology PatnaPatnaIndia

Personalised recommendations