Skip to main content

The Influence of Visual, Auditory, and Linguistic Cues on Children’s Novel Verb Generalization

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
  • 210 Accesses

Abstract

An influential theory proposes that children learn words by initially relying on perceptual cues to identify referents, only later recruiting social or linguistic cues to acquire new words (Golinkoff and Hirsh-Pasek 2007). Here we ask whether a similar bias also characterizes children’s extensions of words to novel contexts. In order to establish relative preference for cues in different modalities, we focus on how children recruit auditory, visual, and lexical distributional cues in extending verbs to unfamiliar contexts involving missing or conflicting information regarding the referent of the verb. Four-year olds were taught two nonce verbs and subsequently asked to produce one of the two verbs in novel contexts involving conflicting cues (e.g., the linguistic cue associated with one verb is paired with the visual cue associated with the other verb). To assess relative cue preference, children’s responses in the ‘bimodal conflicting cues’ condition were compared with their responses in a ‘baseline unimodal’ condition in which only one of the three cues was presented. Four-year olds overwhelmingly prefer to produce verbs associated with visual cues versus auditory or linguistic cues. But there is no advantage for auditory over linguistic cues when they are placed in conflict. These findings demonstrate that children weight perceptual cues more heavily than linguistic cues during the process of verb generalization, but only if the cues are visual. The cue-weighting strategies that children recruit during verb generalization are similar in some respects to those proposed for the process of mapping word forms onto referents during word learning and additionally may be driven by visual dominance effects that characterize cognitive processing in other kinds of tasks (Colavita 1974).

We gratefully acknowledge the cooperation of the Tiny Minders Preschool, Louisville Public Library, and Gateway Amusement Park in allowing us to recruit children to participate in our research, as well as the many children and parents who took part in our study at the Language, Development, and Cognition Lab.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

References

  • Booth, A., and S. Waxman. 2002. Word learning is ‘smart’: Evidence that conceptual information affects preschoolers’ extension of novel words. Cognition 84: B11–B22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., and E. Bartlett. 1978. Acquiring a single new word. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 15: 17–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, E.V. 2004. How language acquisition builds on cognitive development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8: 472–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Colavita, F.B. 1974. Human sensory dominance. Perception and Psychophysics 16: 409–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R.M., and K. Hirsh-Pasek. 2007. Language development: The view from the radical middle. [Keynote address.] In Proceedings of the 31st Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, ed. H. Caunt-Nulton, S. Kulatilake, and I. Woo, 1–25. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golinkoff, R.M., C.B. Mervis, and K. Hirsh-Pasek. 1994. Early object labels: The case for a developmental lexical principles framework. Journal of Child Language 21: 125–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M., D. Gentner, and N. Uchida. 1994. Children’s theories of word meaning: The role of shape similarity in early acquisition. Cognitive Development 9: 45–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johanson, M., and A. Papafragou. 2011. Effects of labels on children’s category boundaries. In Proceedings from the 35th Annual Boston University on Language Development, ed. N. Danis, K. Mesh, and H. Sung, 334–346. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemler-Nelson, D.G. 1995. Principle-based inferences in young children’s categorization: Revisiting the impact of function on the naming of artifacts. Cognitive Development 10: 347–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., and E. Shipley. 2001. Labelling patterns and object naming. Developmental Psychology 4: 109–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, R.M., and C. Fisher. 2009. 2-year-olds use distributional cues to interpret transitivity-alternating verbs. Language and Cognitive Processes 24: 777–803.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, L.B., S.S. Jones, and B. Landau. 1992. Count nouns, adjectives, and perceptual properties in children’s novel word interpretations. Developmental Psychology 28: 273–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. 1995. Pragmatic contexts for early verb learning. In Beyond Names for Things: Young Children’s Acquisition of Verbs, ed. M. Tomasello, and W.E. Merriman, 115–146. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bhuvana Narasimhan .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Nature Singapore Pte Ltd.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Narasimhan, B., Cheng, F., Davidson, P., Kan, P.F., Wagner, M. (2017). The Influence of Visual, Auditory, and Linguistic Cues on Children’s Novel Verb Generalization. In: Sengupta, G., Sircar, S., Raman, M., Balusu, R. (eds) Perspectives on the Architecture and Acquisition of Syntax. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-4295-9_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-4295-9_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-10-4294-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-10-4295-9

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics