Skip to main content

Supporting Concordant Intersubjectivity and Sense of ‘Belonging’ for Under Three-Year-Olds in Early Years Settings

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Under-three Year Olds in Policy and Practice

Abstract

Through concordant intersubjective interactions, in which mutual consciousness is supported in positive companionship (Minnis H, Marwick H, Arthur J, McLaughlin A. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 15(6):336–342, 2006; Trevarthen C. The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In: Braten S (eds) Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 15–46, 1998; Trevarthen C. Infant Child Dev 20(1): 119–135, 2011), across the first months and years, infants and young children develop their understanding of themselves and other people. Familiar shared experiences, playful interactions, and co-creation of meanings develop their growing understanding of emotionality and intentionality in themselves and others, and their expectations about other people’s acts and feelings (Marwick H, Murray L. The effects of maternal depression on the ‘musicality’ of infant directed speech and conversational engagement. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds) Communicative musicality: narratives of expressive gesture and being human. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 281–300, 2008), and lead to a growing sense of self-identity in relation to others, which brings with it a reliable sense of ‘belonging’, or ‘awareness of a collective level of knowing’, and meaning within their interpersonal world (Gratier M, Trevarthen C. J Conscious Stud 15(10–11): 122–158, 2008). This positive confidence in understanding of self and other can become vulnerable in the transition into, and experience of, the group environment of an early years setting, in which existing expectations, perspectives and intentions of the participants may contrast and vary, and lead to discordant intersubjective experience of communication and shared understandings for a child (Marwick H, Murray L. The effects of maternal depression on the ‘musicality’ of infant directed speech and conversational engagement. In: Malloch S, Trevarthen C (eds) Communicative musicality: narratives of expressive gesture and being human. Oxford University Press, Oxford, pp. 281–300, 2008). This chapter examines the challenges inherent in supporting concordant intersubjectivity and a sense of ‘belonging’ for under –3-year-olds in group based infant and toddler settings, and models of pedagogy and interaction applied in such settings.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Ainsworth, M. (1985). Patterns of mother-infant attachments: Antecedents and effects on development. Bulletin of New York Academy of Medicine, 66(9), 771–790.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anghileri, J. (2006). Scaffolding practices that enhance mathematics learning. Journal of Mathematics Teacher Education, 9, 33–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bornstein, M. H., Haynes, O. M., O’Reilly, A. W., & Painter, K. M. (1996). Solitary and collaborative pretense play in early childhood: Sources of individual variation in the development of representational competence. Child Development, 67(6), 2910–2929.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: The early mother-infant interaction. In M. Lewis & L. A. Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caretaker. New York/London: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1983). Child’s talk. Learning to use language. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1986). Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1995). From joint attention to the meeting of minds: An introduction. In C. Moore & P. J. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origin and role in development. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buber, M. (1947). Between man and man (Translation by Ronald G. Smith). London: Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Buckley, B. (2003). Children’s communicative skills from birth to five. Oxon: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R., & Aslin, R. (1990). Preference for infant-directed speech in the first month after birth. Child Development, 61, 1584–1595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dalli, C., White, E. J., Rockel, J., Duhn, I., with Buchanan, E., Davidson, S., Ganly, S., Kus, L., & Wang, B. (2011). Quality early childhood education for under-two-year-olds: What should it look like? A literature review. Report to the Ministry of Education. http://www.educationcounts.govt.nz/

  • DeCasper, A. J., & Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother’s voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emde, R., Kubicek, L., & Oppenheim, D. (1997). Imaginative reality observed during early language development. The International Journal of Psychoanalysis, 78(Pt 1), 115–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fein, G. G. (1984). The self-building potential of pretend play or “I got a fish, all by myself”. In T. D. Yawkey & A. D. Pellegrini (Eds.), Child’s play: Developmental and applied. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A. (1992). Meaningful melodies in mothers’ speech to infants. In H. Papousek, U. Jürgens, & M. Papousek (Eds.), Nonverbal vocal communication: Comparative and developmental aspects (pp. 262–282). Cambridge/Paris: Cambridge University Press/Editions de la Maison des Sciences de l’Homme.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernald, A., & Kuhl, P. (1987). Acoustic determinants of infant preference for motherese speech. Infant Behaviour and Development, 10, 279–293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, J. (2016). Interacting or interfering? Improving interactions in the early years. Maidenhead/Berkshire: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goouch, K., & Powell, S. (2013). The baby room, principles, policy and practice. Maidenhead/Berkshire: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gratier, M., & Trevarthen, C. (2008). Musical narrative and motives for culture in mother-infant vocal interaction. The Journal of Consciousness Studies, 15(10–11), 122–158.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and semantics (Vol. 3, pp. 41–58). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habermas, J. (1970). Introductory remarks to a theory of communicative competence. Repreinted in Dreitzel, H. P. (Ed.), Recent sociology, No. 2. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1975). Learning how to mean: Explorations in the development of language. London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Horgan, D. (1981). Learning to tell jokes: A case study of metalinguistic abilities. Journal of Child Language, 8, 217–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malloch, S., & Trevarthen, C. (Eds.). (2008). Communicative musicality: Narratives of expressive gesture and being human (pp. 281–300). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marwick, H. (2016). Intersubjectivity and supportiveness in children’s conversations with each other in pre-school settings. Petite Enfance: socialisation et transitions. International Colloquium, Villetaneuse, France. https://hal-univ-paris13.archives-ouvertes.fr/PETITE-ENFANCE

  • Marwick, H., & Murray, L. (2008). The effects of maternal depression on the ‘musicality’ of infant directed speech and conversational engagement. In S. Malloch & C. Trevarthen (Eds.), Communicative musicality: Narratives of expressive gesture and being human (pp. 281–300). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marwick, H., Doolin, O., Allely, C. S., McConnachie, A., Johnson, P., Puckering, C., Golding, J., Gillberg, C., & Wilson, P. (2013). Predictors of diagnosis of child psychiatric disorder in adult-infant social-communicative interaction at 12 months. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 34, 562–572.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1994). Imitation, memory, and the representation of persons. Infant Behavior & Development, 17, 83–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Minnis, H., Marwick, H., Arthur, J., & McLaughlin, A. (2006). Reactive attachment disorder – A theoretical model beyond attachment. European Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 15(6), 336–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L. (1992). The impact of post-natal depression on infant development. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 543–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1985). Emotional regulation of interactions between two-month-olds and their mothers. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 177–197). Norwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., & Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant’s role in mother-infant communication. Journal of Child Language, 13, 15–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murray, L., Marwick, H., & Arteche, A. (2010). Sadness in mothers’ ‘baby talk’ predicts affective disorder in adolescent offspring. Infant Behaviour and Development, 33, 361–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J., & PezŽ, A. (1993). Immediate imitation as a basis for primary communication in toddlers and autistic children. In J. Nadel & L. Camioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early communicative development (pp. 139–156). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Réserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2(2), 164–173.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, E., & Molnár, P. (2004). Homo imitans or Homo provocans? Human imprinting model of neonatal imitation. Infant Behavior and Development, 27, 54–63.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2015). Enrolment in childcare and pre-school: OECD Family Database. OECD Publishing. http://www.oecd.org/els/family/database.htm

  • Osofsky, J. D. (1987). Handbook of infant development (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papousek, M., & Papousek, H. (1981). Musical elements in the infant’s vocalisation: Their significance for communication, cognition, and creativity. In L. P. Lipsitt & C. K. Rovee-Collier (Eds.), Advances in infancy research (Vol. 1, pp. 163–224).

    Google Scholar 

  • Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1997). Fragile aspects of early social interaction. In L. Murray & P. J. Cooper (Eds.), Postpartum depression and child development (pp. 35–53). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papousek, M., Papousek, H., & Bornstein, M. (1985). The naturalistic vocal environment of young infants: On the significance of homogeneity and variability in parental speech. In T. M. Field & N. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 269–297). Noorwood: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peers, C., & Fleer, M. (2014). The theory of ‘belonging’: Defining concepts used within belonging, being and becoming – The Australian early years learning framework. Educational Philosophy and Theory, 46(8), 914–928.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Puckering, C., Allely, C. S., Doolin, O., Purves, D., McConnachie, A., Johnson, P., Marwick, H., Heron, J., Golding, J., Gillberg, C., & Wilson, P. (2014). Association between parent-infant interactions in infancy and disruptive behaviour disorders at age seven. A nested case–control ALSPAC study. BMC Pediatrics, 14, 223.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (1991). Playing with others’ expectations: Teasing and mucking about in the first year. In A. Whiten (Ed.), Natural theories of mind. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ryan, J. (1974). Early language development: Towards a communicational analysis. In M. P. M. Richards (Ed.), The integration of a child into a social world (pp. 185–213). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singer, E., Nederend, M., Penninx, L., Tajik, M., & Boom, J. (2014). The teacher’s role in supporting young children’s level of play engagement. Early Child Development and Care, 184(8), 1233–1249.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (1985/2000). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and development psychology. (Second Edition, with new Introduction). New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D., Spieker, S., & Mackain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelinguistic infants. Developmental Psychology, 18, 727–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stratigos, T., Bradley, B., & Sumsion, J. (2014). Infants, family day care and the politics of belonging. International Journal of Early Childhood, 46(2), 171–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., Chen, L. A., & Bornstein, M. H. (1998). Mothers’ knowledge about children’s play and language development: Short-term stability and interrelations. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 115–124.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tizard, B., & Hughes, H. (2002). Young children learning (2nd ed.). Malden: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1974, May 2). Conversations with a two-month old. New Scientist, 230–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy. A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1998). The concept and foundations of infant intersubjectivity. In S. Braten (Ed.), Intersubjective communication and emotion in early ontogeny (pp. 15–46). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1999). Musicality and the intrinsic motive pulse: Evidence from human psychobiology and infant communication. Musicae Scientae, Special Issue, 3, 155–215.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (2011). What is it like to be a person who knows nothing? Defining the active intersubjective mind of a newborn human being. Infant and Child Development, 20(1), 119–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Aitken, K. (2001, January). Infant intersubjectivity: Research, theory, and clinical applications. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(1), 348. Review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture and symbol (pp. 183–229). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Marwick, H. (1982). Cooperative understanding in infancy. Project Report to the Spencer Foundation. Chicago: University of Edinburgh.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Marwick, H. (1986). Signs of motivation for speech in infants, and the nature of a mother’s support for development of language. In B. Lindblom & R. Zetterstrom (Eds.), Precursors of early speech (pp. 279–308). Basingstoke: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher mental processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, E. J. (2014). Are you ‘Avin a Laff?’: A pedagogical response to Bakhtinian carnivalesque in early childhood education. Educational Philosophy and Theory: Incorporating ACCESS Volume 46, Issue 8, Special issue: Philosophy and Pedagogy of Early Childhood. doi:10.1080/00131857.2013.781497.

  • White, E. J., & Redder, B. (2015). Proximity with under two-year-olds in early childhood education: A silent pedagogical encounter. Early Education and Care. doi:10.1080/03004430.2015.1028386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • White, E. J., Peter, M., & Redder, B. (2015). Infant and teacher dialogue in education and care: A pedagogical imperative. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 30, 160–173. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.10.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winnicott, D. (1971). Playing and reality. London: Tavistock.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Helen Marwick .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Science+Business Media Singapore

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Marwick, H. (2017). Supporting Concordant Intersubjectivity and Sense of ‘Belonging’ for Under Three-Year-Olds in Early Years Settings. In: White, E.J., Dalli, C. (eds) Under-three Year Olds in Policy and Practice. Policy and Pedagogy with Under-three Year Olds: Cross-disciplinary Insights and Innovations. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-2275-3_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-2275-3_7

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-10-2274-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-10-2275-3

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics