Advertisement

Reading Against the Stream: Using the Wrong Strategies to Acquire L2 Literacy

  • Clay WilliamsEmail author
Chapter
  • 701 Downloads
Part of the Springer Texts in Education book series (SPTE)

Abstract

This chapter expands upon the idea of how script-specific word level decoding strategies in the L1 can negatively impact the development of word-level decoding skills in a foreign language whose script favors different word-decoding strategies. The author describes a case study of low-level proficiency Taiwanese students of English whose attempts at English literacy acquisition were being stymied by their lack of awareness that the new script required the development of altogether different processing styles, as well as the efforts undertaken to ameliorate the students’ L2 reading abilities. Other cases from the literature are used to build a case for the prevalence of this word-level processing problem.

Keywords

Phonological Awareness Chinese Student Phonemic Awareness Chinese Script English Orthography 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Ackert, P., & Lee, L. (2005). Reading and vocabulary development 2: Thoughts & notions. Boston, MA: Thomson Heinle.Google Scholar
  2. Byrne, B., Freebody, P., & Gates, A. (1992). Longitudinal data on the relation of word-reading strategies to comprehension, reading time and phonemic awareness. Reading, Research Quarterly, 27, 141–151.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Cheung, H. (1999). Improving phonological awareness and word reading in a later learned alphabetic script. Cognition, 70, 1–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Cheung, H., & Chen, H. C. (2004). Early orthographic experience modifies both phonological awareness and on-line speech processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 19(1), 1–28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Cheung, H., Chen, H. C., Lai, C. Y., Wong, O. C., & Hills, M. (2001). The development of phonological awareness: Effects of spoken language experience and orthography. Cognition, 81(3), 227–241.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Cipollone, N., Keiser, S. H., & Vasishth, S. (1998). Language files (7th ed., pp. 448–456). Columbus, OH: The Ohio State University Press.Google Scholar
  7. Holm, A., & Dodd, B. (1996). The effect of first written language on the acquisition of English literacy. Cognition, 59, 119–147.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Lessow-Hurley, J. (1990). The foundations of dual language instruction. New York: Longman.Google Scholar
  9. McDowell, H., & Lorch, M. P. (2008). Phonemic awareness in Chinese L1 readers of English: Not simply an effect of orthography. TESOL Quarterly, 42(3), 495–513.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  10. Read, C., Zhang, Y. F., Nie, H. Y., & Ding, B. Q. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic writing. Cognition, 24, 31–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Sun, B., Zhou, H., & Zhu, B. X. (2013). Effects of English phonological awareness training on Chinese child EFL learners’ literacy development. Canadian Social Science, 9(1), 56–64.Google Scholar
  12. Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2003). Alphabetic readers quickly acquire orthographic structure in learning to read Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(2), 183–208.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  13. Wiley, K. (2002). Fast track phonics: For young adults and adults. White Plains, NY: Pearson Education.Google Scholar
  14. Williams, C. H. (2010). Chinese literacy-learning strategy impact on English reading development: A case study of Taiwanese learners of English. Asian EFL Journal, 41, 3–23.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Singapore 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Akita International UniversityAkitaJapan

Personalised recommendations