Advertisement

Chinese on the Brain

  • Clay WilliamsEmail author
Chapter
  • 697 Downloads
Part of the Springer Texts in Education book series (SPTE)

Abstract

This chapter reviews the relevant literature on how Chinese script is processed at the character level. Basic word decoding processes (used in any written script) are reviewed, along with language-specific accommodations made to the basic model for the case of Chinese. Confirmatory evidence of these hypotheses on word identification are reviewed, including the literature on reading impairments such as dyslexia and language-specific neurological differences seen in Chinese readers. The basic means of character identification (through character-internal radical analysis) is discussed, and evidence from the literature on psycholinguistic studies including priming and fMRI studies is used to confirm this as the basis for character-level processing in Chinese, and indeed, reveals Chinese reading as quite unique compared with alphabetically transcribed languages. Other salient differences between Chinese processing and that of other languages are discussed, such as the difficulty in defining “words” and cerebral asymmetries vis-à-vis with reading in other languages.

Keywords

Chinese Character Dyslexic Child Semantic Radical Chinese Reader Phonetic Radical 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Akamatsu, N. (2005). Effects of second language reading proficiency and first language orthography on second language word recognition. In V. Cook & B. Bassetti (Eds.), Second language writing systems (pp. 238–259). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.Google Scholar
  2. Andrews, S. (1997). The effects of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychological Bulletin & Review, 4, 439–461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Bassetti, B. (2005). Effects of writing systems on second language awareness: Word awareness in English learners of Chinese as a foreign language. In V. Cook & B. Bassetti (Eds.), Second language writing systems (pp. 335–356). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.Google Scholar
  4. Bi, Y., Han, Z., Weekes, B., & Shu, H. (2007). The interaction between semantic and the nonsemantic systems in reading: Evidence from Chinese. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2660–2673.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Biederman, I., & Tsao, Y. C. (1979). On processing Chinese ideographs and English words: Some implications from Stroop-test results. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 125–132.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Carreiras, M., Perea, M., & Grainger, J. (1997). Effects of orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 857–871.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Chung, K. K. H. (2006). Exploring the reading–writing connection in Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Reading and Writing, 19(6), 543–561.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Chen, H. C. (1987). Character detection in reading Chinese: Effects of context and display format. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 29, 45–50.Google Scholar
  9. Chen, H. C. (1996). Chinese reading and comprehension: A cognitive psychology perspective. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Chen, H. C. (1999). How do readers of Chinese process words during reading for comprehension? In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 257–278). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  11. Chen, Y. P., Allport, D. A., & Marshall, J. C. (1996). What are the functional orthographic units in Chinese word recognition: The stroke or the stroke pattern? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 49(4), 1024–1043.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chen, K. J., & Liu, S. H. (1992). Word identification for Mandarin Chinese sentences. In: Proceedings of the Fifteenth International Conference on Computational Linguistics (pp. 101–107), Nantes.Google Scholar
  13. Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151–216). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  14. Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. F., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention & Performance VI (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  15. Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Ding, G., Peng, D., & Taft, M. (2004). The nature of the mental representation of radicals in Chinese: A priming study. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 530–539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dong, Y., Nakamura, K., Okada, T., Hanakawa, T., Fukuyama, H., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2005). Neural mechanisms underlying the processing of Chinese words: An fMRI study. Neuroscience Research, 52, 139–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1999). Semantic radicals in phonetic compounds: Implications for visual character recognition in Chinese. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.) Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 19–35). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  19. Flores d’Arcais, G. B. (1992). Graphemic, phonological and semantic activation processes during the recognition of Chinese characters. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 37–66). Amsterdam: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Forster, K. I., & Shen, D. (1996). No enemies in the neighborhood: Absence of inhibitory effects in lexical decision and categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 696–713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Grainger, J. (1990). Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 228–244.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(54), doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054
  24. Hatta, T. (1977). Recognition of Japanese kanji in the left and right visual fields. Neuropsychologia, 15(4–5), 685–688.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Ho, C. S. H. (2003). Reading acquisition and developmental dyslexia in Chinese: A cognitive perspective. In N. Goulandris (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons (pp. 277–296). Philadelphia, PA: Whurr Publishers.Google Scholar
  26. Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K. H., Lee, S. H., & Tsang, S. M. (2007). In search of subtypes of Chinese developmental dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 61–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ho, C. S. H., & Lai, D. N. C. (1999). Naming-speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 173–186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Ho, C. S. H., Law, T. P.-S., & Ng, P. M. (2000). The phonological deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 13, 57–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Hoosain, R. (1992). Psychological reality of the word in Chinese. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 111–130). Elsevier, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
  30. Huang, H. S., & Zhang, H. R. (1997). An analysis of phonemic awareness, word awareness and tone awareness among dyslexia children. Bulletin of Special Education and Rehabilitation, 5, 125–138.Google Scholar
  31. Lee, S. Y., Stigler, J. W., & Stevenson, H. W. (1986). Beginning reading in Chinese and English. In B. R. Foorman & A. W. Siegel (Eds.), Acquisition of reading skills: Cultural constraints and cognitive universals. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.Google Scholar
  32. Liu, I. M. (1983). Cueing function of fragments of Chinese characters in reading. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 25, 85–90.Google Scholar
  33. Marshall, J. C., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175–199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. McClelland, J. L., & Rummelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375–407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  35. McWhorter, J. (2011). What language is: And what it isn’t and what it could be. New York: Gotham.Google Scholar
  36. Miao, X. C. (1999). Sentence understanding in Chinese. In J., Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 279–295). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  37. Packard, V. (1957). The hidden persuaders. Brooklyn, NY: Pocket Books.Google Scholar
  38. Packard, J. L. (1998). Introduction. In J. L. Packard (Ed.), New approaches to Chinese word formation: Morphology, phonology and the Lexicon in modern and ancient Chinese (pp. 1–34). Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Packard, J. (2000). The morphology of Chinese. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Papp, K. H., Newsome, S. L., & Noel, R. W. (1984). Word shape’s in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 413–428.Google Scholar
  41. Patterson, K. E., & Morton, J. (1985). From orthography to phonology: An attempt at an old interpretation. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface Dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 335–359). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  42. Peng, D. L., Li, Y. P., & Liu, Z. Z. (1994). Identification of the Chinese two character word under repetition priming condition. ACTA Psychologica Sinica, 26, 393–400.Google Scholar
  43. Peng, D. L., Lui, Y., & Wang, C. M. (1999). How is access representation organized? The relation of polymorphemic words and their morphemes in Chinese. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 65–89). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  44. Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 101–118.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. (1995). Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 633–643.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Perfetti, C. A., Zhang, S., & Berent, I. (1992). Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a ‘universal’ phonological principle. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 227–248). Amsterdam: North Holland.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Rozin, P., Poritsky, S., & Sotsky, R. (1971). American children with reading problems can easily learn to read English represented in Chinese characters. Science, 171, 1264–1267.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). The time course of information activation and utilization in visual word recognition. In D. Besner, T. G. Waller, & E. M. MacKinnon (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 199–252). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  49. Shen, D., & Forster, K. (1999). Masked phonological priming in reading Chinese words depends on the task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5/6), 429–459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  50. Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32(1), 78–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  51. Shu, H., Meng, X. Z., Chen, X., Luan, H., & Cao, F. (2005). The subtypes of developmental dyslexia in Chinese: Evidence from three cases. Dyslexia, 11(4), 311–329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Siok, W. T., Niu, Z. D., Jin, Z., Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (2008). A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(14), 5561–5566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Sun, F. C., Morita, M., & Stark, L. W. (1985). Comparative patterns of reading eye movement in Chinese and English. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 502–506.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  54. Taft, M., Liu, Y., & Zhu, X. P. (1999). Morphemic processing in reading Chinese. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  55. Tan, L. H., Hoosain, R., & Siok, W. W. T. (1996). Activation of phonological codes before access to character meaning in written Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 43–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Taylor, I., & Taylor, M. M. (1983). The psychology of reading. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
  57. Tham, W. W. P., Liow, S. J. R., Rajapakse, J. C., Leong, T. C., Ng, S. E. S., Lim, W. E. H., et al. (2005). Phonological processing in Chinese–English bilingual biscriptals: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 28(3), 579–587.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  58. Wang, S. C. (1998). A study on the learning and teaching of hanzi—Chinese characters. Working Papers in Educational Linguistics 14(1), 69–102.Google Scholar
  59. Weekes, B. S., Chen, M. J., & Yin, W.-G. (1997a). Anomia without dyslexia in Chinese. Neurocase, 3, 51–60.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Weekes, B. S., Coltheart, M., & Gordon, E. V. (1997b). Deep dyslexia and right hemisphere reading: A regional cerebral blood-flow study. Aphasiology, 11, 1139–1158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  61. Williams, C. H., & Bever, T. (2010). Chinese character decoding: a semantic bias? Reading and Writing, 23(5), 589–605.Google Scholar
  62. Woo, E. Y. C., & Hoosain, R. (1984). Visual and auditory functions of Chinese dyslexics. Psychologia, 27, 164–170.Google Scholar
  63. Wu, C. Y., Ho, M. H., & Chen, S. H. (2012). A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing. Neuroimage, 63(1), 381–391.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  64. Wu, J. T., & Liu, I. M. (1997). Phonological activation in pronouncing characters. In H. C. Chen (Ed.), The cognitive processing of Chinese and related Asian languages (pp. 47–64). Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.Google Scholar
  65. Yin, W. G., & Weekes, B. S. (2003). Dyslexia in Chinese: Clues from cognitive neuropsychology. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 255–279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Zhang, B. Y., & Peng, D. L. (1992). Decomposed storage in the Chinese lexicon. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese. Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
  67. Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26(5), 1245–1265.Google Scholar
  68. Zhou, X., Shu, H., Bi, Y., & Shi, D. (1999). Is there phonologically mediated access to lexical semantics in reading Chinese? In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 135–171). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.Google Scholar
  69. Zhu, Y. P. (1987). Analysis of cueing functions of the phonetic in modern Chinese. Unpublished paper, East China Normal University (in Chinese)Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Singapore 2016

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Akita International UniversityAkitaJapan

Personalised recommendations