Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Springer Texts in Education ((SPTE))

  • 967 Accesses

Abstract

This chapter reviews the relevant literature on how Chinese script is processed at the character level. Basic word decoding processes (used in any written script) are reviewed, along with language-specific accommodations made to the basic model for the case of Chinese. Confirmatory evidence of these hypotheses on word identification are reviewed, including the literature on reading impairments such as dyslexia and language-specific neurological differences seen in Chinese readers. The basic means of character identification (through character-internal radical analysis) is discussed, and evidence from the literature on psycholinguistic studies including priming and fMRI studies is used to confirm this as the basis for character-level processing in Chinese, and indeed, reveals Chinese reading as quite unique compared with alphabetically transcribed languages. Other salient differences between Chinese processing and that of other languages are discussed, such as the difficulty in defining “words” and cerebral asymmetries vis-à-vis with reading in other languages.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    I’ve asked such to many of my students—both graduate and undergraduate—and this is a pretty close summary of the normal responses.

  2. 2.

    Although in the case of text reading, semantic information certainly does inform lexical search as the reader can often make reasonable predictions about upcoming content.

  3. 3.

    Which is, admittedly, still the lexical route, being an analysis of orthography, but it is informed by semantics prelexically.

  4. 4.

    And this is actually even more complicated by the prevailing tendency for people to say “want to” as “wanna”.

References

  • Akamatsu, N. (2005). Effects of second language reading proficiency and first language orthography on second language word recognition. In V. Cook & B. Bassetti (Eds.), Second language writing systems (pp. 238–259). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, S. (1997). The effects of orthographic similarity on lexical retrieval: Resolving neighborhood conflicts. Psychological Bulletin & Review, 4, 439–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bassetti, B. (2005). Effects of writing systems on second language awareness: Word awareness in English learners of Chinese as a foreign language. In V. Cook & B. Bassetti (Eds.), Second language writing systems (pp. 335–356). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bi, Y., Han, Z., Weekes, B., & Shu, H. (2007). The interaction between semantic and the nonsemantic systems in reading: Evidence from Chinese. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2660–2673.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biederman, I., & Tsao, Y. C. (1979). On processing Chinese ideographs and English words: Some implications from Stroop-test results. Cognitive Psychology, 11, 125–132.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, M., Perea, M., & Grainger, J. (1997). Effects of orthographic neighborhood in visual word recognition: Cross-task comparisons. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 857–871.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan, D. W., Ho, C. S. H., Tsang, S. M., Lee, S. H., & Chung, K. K. H. (2006). Exploring the reading–writing connection in Chinese children with dyslexia in Hong Kong. Reading and Writing, 19(6), 543–561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H. C. (1987). Character detection in reading Chinese: Effects of context and display format. Chinese Journal of Psychology, 29, 45–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H. C. (1996). Chinese reading and comprehension: A cognitive psychology perspective. In M. H. Bond (Ed.), The handbook of Chinese psychology. Oxford, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, H. C. (1999). How do readers of Chinese process words during reading for comprehension? In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 257–278). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, Y. P., Allport, D. A., & Marshall, J. C. (1996). What are the functional orthographic units in Chinese word recognition: The stroke or the stroke pattern? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 49(4), 1024–1043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, K. J., & Liu, S. H. (1992). Word identification for Mandarin Chinese sentences. In: Proceedings of the Fifteenth International Conference on Computational Linguistics (pp. 101–107), Nantes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151–216). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J. F., & Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention & Performance VI (pp. 535–555). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, G., Peng, D., & Taft, M. (2004). The nature of the mental representation of radicals in Chinese: A priming study. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 530–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dong, Y., Nakamura, K., Okada, T., Hanakawa, T., Fukuyama, H., Mazziotta, J. C., et al. (2005). Neural mechanisms underlying the processing of Chinese words: An fMRI study. Neuroscience Research, 52, 139–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feldman, L. B., & Siok, W. W. T. (1999). Semantic radicals in phonetic compounds: Implications for visual character recognition in Chinese. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.) Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 19–35). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flores d’Arcais, G. B. (1992). Graphemic, phonological and semantic activation processes during the recognition of Chinese characters. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 37–66). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, K. I., & Shen, D. (1996). No enemies in the neighborhood: Absence of inhibitory effects in lexical decision and categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 696–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J. (1990). Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming. Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 228–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J., & Ziegler, J. C. (2011). A dual-route approach to orthographic processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2(54), doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00054

  • Hatta, T. (1977). Recognition of Japanese kanji in the left and right visual fields. Neuropsychologia, 15(4–5), 685–688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H. (2003). Reading acquisition and developmental dyslexia in Chinese: A cognitive perspective. In N. Goulandris (Ed.), Dyslexia in different languages: Cross-linguistic comparisons (pp. 277–296). Philadelphia, PA: Whurr Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W., Chung, K. K. H., Lee, S. H., & Tsang, S. M. (2007). In search of subtypes of Chinese developmental dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., & Lai, D. N. C. (1999). Naming-speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 173–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., Law, T. P.-S., & Ng, P. M. (2000). The phonological deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 13, 57–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoosain, R. (1992). Psychological reality of the word in Chinese. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese (pp. 111–130). Elsevier, Philadelphia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H. S., & Zhang, H. R. (1997). An analysis of phonemic awareness, word awareness and tone awareness among dyslexia children. Bulletin of Special Education and Rehabilitation, 5, 125–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee, S. Y., Stigler, J. W., & Stevenson, H. W. (1986). Beginning reading in Chinese and English. In B. R. Foorman & A. W. Siegel (Eds.), Acquisition of reading skills: Cultural constraints and cognitive universals. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, I. M. (1983). Cueing function of fragments of Chinese characters in reading. Acta Psychologica Taiwanica, 25, 85–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marshall, J. C., & Newcombe, F. (1973). Patterns of paralexia: A psycholinguistic approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2, 175–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McClelland, J. L., & Rummelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 1. An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McWhorter, J. (2011). What language is: And what it isn’t and what it could be. New York: Gotham.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miao, X. C. (1999). Sentence understanding in Chinese. In J., Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 279–295). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Packard, V. (1957). The hidden persuaders. Brooklyn, NY: Pocket Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Packard, J. L. (1998). Introduction. In J. L. Packard (Ed.), New approaches to Chinese word formation: Morphology, phonology and the Lexicon in modern and ancient Chinese (pp. 1–34). Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Packard, J. (2000). The morphology of Chinese. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Papp, K. H., Newsome, S. L., & Noel, R. W. (1984). Word shape’s in poor shape for the race to the lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 413–428.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, K. E., & Morton, J. (1985). From orthography to phonology: An attempt at an old interpretation. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marshall, & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface Dyslexia: Neuropsychological and cognitive studies of phonological reading (pp. 335–359). Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peng, D. L., Li, Y. P., & Liu, Z. Z. (1994). Identification of the Chinese two character word under repetition priming condition. ACTA Psychologica Sinica, 26, 393–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peng, D. L., Lui, Y., & Wang, C. M. (1999). How is access representation organized? The relation of polymorphemic words and their morphemes in Chinese. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 65–89). Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (1998). The time course of graphic, phonological, and semantic activation in Chinese character identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24(1), 101–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Zhang, S. (1995). Very early phonological activation in Chinese reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 17, 633–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Zhang, S., & Berent, I. (1992). Reading in English and Chinese: Evidence for a ‘universal’ phonological principle. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (pp. 227–248). Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P., Poritsky, S., & Sotsky, R. (1971). American children with reading problems can easily learn to read English represented in Chinese characters. Science, 171, 1264–1267.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). The time course of information activation and utilization in visual word recognition. In D. Besner, T. G. Waller, & E. M. MacKinnon (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 199–252). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shen, D., & Forster, K. (1999). Masked phonological priming in reading Chinese words depends on the task. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14(5/6), 429–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32(1), 78–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Meng, X. Z., Chen, X., Luan, H., & Cao, F. (2005). The subtypes of developmental dyslexia in Chinese: Evidence from three cases. Dyslexia, 11(4), 311–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siok, W. T., Niu, Z. D., Jin, Z., Perfetti, C. A., & Tan, L. H. (2008). A structural-functional basis for dyslexia in the cortex of Chinese readers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105(14), 5561–5566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sun, F. C., Morita, M., & Stark, L. W. (1985). Comparative patterns of reading eye movement in Chinese and English. Perception and Psychophysics, 37, 502–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taft, M., Liu, Y., & Zhu, X. P. (1999). Morphemic processing in reading Chinese. In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., Hoosain, R., & Siok, W. W. T. (1996). Activation of phonological codes before access to character meaning in written Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 43–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, I., & Taylor, M. M. (1983). The psychology of reading. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tham, W. W. P., Liow, S. J. R., Rajapakse, J. C., Leong, T. C., Ng, S. E. S., Lim, W. E. H., et al. (2005). Phonological processing in Chinese–English bilingual biscriptals: An fMRI study. NeuroImage, 28(3), 579–587.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S. C. (1998). A study on the learning and teaching of hanzi—Chinese characters. Working Papers in Educational Linguistics 14(1), 69–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S., Chen, M. J., & Yin, W.-G. (1997a). Anomia without dyslexia in Chinese. Neurocase, 3, 51–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S., Coltheart, M., & Gordon, E. V. (1997b). Deep dyslexia and right hemisphere reading: A regional cerebral blood-flow study. Aphasiology, 11, 1139–1158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Williams, C. H., & Bever, T. (2010). Chinese character decoding: a semantic bias? Reading and Writing, 23(5), 589–605.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woo, E. Y. C., & Hoosain, R. (1984). Visual and auditory functions of Chinese dyslexics. Psychologia, 27, 164–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu, C. Y., Ho, M. H., & Chen, S. H. (2012). A meta-analysis of fMRI studies on Chinese orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing. Neuroimage, 63(1), 381–391.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, J. T., & Liu, I. M. (1997). Phonological activation in pronouncing characters. In H. C. Chen (Ed.), The cognitive processing of Chinese and related Asian languages (pp. 47–64). Hong Kong: Chinese University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yin, W. G., & Weekes, B. S. (2003). Dyslexia in Chinese: Clues from cognitive neuropsychology. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 255–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, B. Y., & Peng, D. L. (1992). Decomposed storage in the Chinese lexicon. In H. C. Chen & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Language processing in Chinese. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., & Marslen-Wilson, W. (2000). The relative time course of semantic and phonological activation in reading Chinese. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 26(5), 1245–1265.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhou, X., Shu, H., Bi, Y., & Shi, D. (1999). Is there phonologically mediated access to lexical semantics in reading Chinese? In J. Wang, A. W. Inhoff, & H. C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 135–171). NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhu, Y. P. (1987). Analysis of cueing functions of the phonetic in modern Chinese. Unpublished paper, East China Normal University (in Chinese)

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Clay Williams .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Science+Business Media Singapore

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Williams, C. (2016). Chinese on the Brain. In: Teaching English Reading in the Chinese-Speaking World. Springer Texts in Education. Springer, Singapore. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-0643-2_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-981-10-0643-2_5

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Singapore

  • Print ISBN: 978-981-10-0641-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-981-10-0643-2

  • eBook Packages: EducationEducation (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics