Skip to main content

Naming the Living World

From the Infant’s Perception of Animacy to a Child’s Species Concept

  • Chapter
Darwin-Inspired Learning

Abstract

Children have an astonishing ability to relate to, talk about, identify and then name organisms in a mental framework shared with their peers; some even become naturalists and bioscientists as well. In this chapter we outline some of the ideas, supported by research evidence, which contribute to this ontogeny of just one aspect of science capability.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Alexander, R. (2008). Towards dialogic teaching: Rethinking classroom talk. Cambridge, MA: Dialogos.

    Google Scholar 

  • Askham, L. A. (1976). The effects of plants on classification behavior in an outdoor environment. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 13(1), 49–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atran, S., & Medin, D. (2008). The native mind and the cultural construction of nature. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Balmford, A., Clegg, L., Coulson, T., & Taylor, J. (2002). Why conservationists should heed Pokémon. Science, 297(5564), 2367b.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barlow, N. (Ed.). (1958). The autobiography of Charles Darwin 1809–1882. With the original omissions restored. London: Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browne, J. (1995). Charles Darwin: Voyaging. New York, NY: Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., &. Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. New York, NY: John Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual change in childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1873). The autobiography of Charles Darwin 1809-1882. London: Collins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durbin, G., Morris, S., Wilkinson, S., & Corbishley, M. (1990). A teacher’s guide to learning from objects. London: English Heritage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Driver, R., Newton, P., & Osborne, J. (2000). Establishing the norms of scientific argumentation in classrooms. Science Education, 84(3), 287–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R., & Spelke, E. (1981). The development of thoughts about animate and inanimate objects: Implications for research on social cognition. In J. H. Flavell & L. Ross (Ed.), Social cognitive development (pp. 43–66). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. (2005). The essential child: Origins of essentialism in everyday thought. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hey, J. (2001). The mind of the species problem. Trends in Ecology and Evolution, 16(7), 326–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakkola, R. (1997). The development of scientific understanding: children’s construction of their first biological theory. (Unpublished PhD thesis, MIT). Quoted in Slaughter, Jakkola & Carey in M. Siegal & C. Peterson. (1999). Children’s understanding of biology and health. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krapp, A. (1999). Interest, motivation and learning: an educational-psychological perspective. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14(1), 23–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M. (1979). The representation of perceived causal connection (PhD thesis). Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Linnaeus, C. (1735, et sequ to 1758). Systema naturae. English edition (1964). Nieuwkoop: B. De Graaf,

    Google Scholar 

  • Linnaeus, C. (1751). Philosophia botanica. Stockholm: G Kiesewetter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Louv, R. (2005). Last child in the woods: Saving our children from nature-deficit disorder. Chapel Hill, NC: Algonquin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, E. M. (1989). Categorization and naming in children: Problems of induction. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nabhan, G. P., & St Antoine, S. (1995). The loss of floral and faunal story: The extinction of experience. In S. R. Kellert & E. O. Wilson (Ed.), The biophilia hypothesis (pp. 229–250). Washington, DC: Island Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Narli, S., Yorek, N., Sahin, M., & Usak, M. (2010). Can we make definite categorization of student attitudes? Journal of Science and Technology Education, 19, 456–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ogborn, J., Kress, G., Martins, I., & McGillicuddy. K. (1996). Explaining science in the classroom. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, P. G., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2011). What plants and animals do early childhood and primary students name? Where do they see them? Journal of Science Education Technology, 20(5), 630–642.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patrick, P. G., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2013). Zoo talk. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1929). The child’s conception of the world. London: Routledge and Kegan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sanders, D. (2010). All netted together: Is there a need for a cultural consilience in the face of extinction? Kew Bulletin, 65(4), 677–680.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spreece, M., & Brent, S. (1985). Children’s understanding of death: A review of three components of a death concept. Child development, 55(1), 671–686.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schiefele, U. (1991). Interest, learning and motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26, 299–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Springer, K. (1999). How a naive theory of biology is acquired. In M. Siegal & C. Peterson (Eds), Children’s understanding of biology and health. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. P., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2001). Looking for ideas: observation, interpretation and hypothesis making by 12 year old pupils undertaking science investigations. International Journal of Science Education, 23(8), 791–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. P., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2007). Nature tables: Stimulating children’s interest in natural objects. Journal of Biological Education, 41(4), 150–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D. (1995). Talking about animals: Studies of young children visiting zoos, a museum and a farm. (Unpublished PhD thesis). King’s College, London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D. (1998). Boy talk: Girl talk. Is it the same at animal exhibits? International Journal of Science Education, 20(7), 795–811.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2001). Talking about plants: Comments of primary school groups looking at plants as exhibits in a botanical garden. Journal of Biological Education, 36, 27–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D., & Reiss M. J. (1999). Building a model of the environment: How do children see animals? Journal of Biological Education, 33(4), 142–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D., & Ueckert, C. (2011). Early biology: The critical years for learning. Journal of Biological Education, 45(4), 173–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2013). Talking and doing science in the early years. London: David Fulton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D. (in press) Naming and narratives at natural history dioramas. In S. D. Tunncliffe & A. Scheresoi (Eds.), Natural history dioramas: History, construction and educational role. Dordrecht: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tversky, B. (1989). Parts, partonomies and taxonomies. Developmental Psychology, 25, 983–995.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uno, G. (2009). Botanical literacy: What and how should students learn about plants? American Journal of Biology, 96, 1753–1759.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wandersee, J. H., & Schussler, E. E. (2001). Toward a theory of plant blindness. Plant Science Bulletin, 47, 2–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson. E. O. (1984). Biophilia. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Wyhe, J. (2009). Darwin in Cambridge. Cambridge: Christ’s College.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yorek, N., & Narli, S. (2009). Modeling of cognitive structure of uncertain scientific concepts using fuzzy-rough set and intuitionistic fuzzy sets: Example of the life concept. International Journal of Uncertainty, Fuzziness and Knowledge- Based Systems, 17(5), 371–380.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yorek, N., Sahin, M., & Aydin, H. (2009). Are animals ‘more alive’ than plants? Animistic-anthropocentic construction of life concept. Eurasia Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education, 5(4), 369–378.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Sense Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Tomkins, S.P., Tunnicliffe, S.D. (2015). Naming the Living World. In: Boulter, C.J., Reiss, M.J., Sanders, D.L. (eds) Darwin-Inspired Learning. New Directions in Mathematics and Science Education. SensePublishers, Rotterdam. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-6209-833-6_12

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics