Skip to main content

Why Do They Not See What I See?

The Difference Between Knowing How and Knowing That

  • Chapter
Transfer, Transitions and Transformations of Learning

Part of the book series: International Technology Education Studies ((ITES))

Abstract

It is a cold but sunny September day in a forest in the south of Sweden. A university teacher, Eric, is taking his biology students out on an excursion. During the morning they have taken samples of soil, and identified plants, trees and many other things. At this specific moment the group has arrived at a peat bog.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Bahrick, H. P., Bahrick, P. O., & Wittlinger, R. P. (1975). Fifty years of memory for names and faces: A cross-sectional approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 104(1), 54-71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bayles, E. E. (1936). An unemphasized factor in current theories regarding the transfer of training. Journal of Educational Psychology, 27(6), 425-430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, D. C., & Dienes, Z. (1993). Implicit learning: Theoretical and empirical issues. Hove: L. Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Björklund, L.-E. (2007). The intuitive practitioner: Cognitive aspects on the development of expertise. Paper presented at the 13 International Conference on Thinking, Norrköping.

    Google Scholar 

  • Björklund, L. (2008a). From novice to expert: Practical wisdom, implications för education from a cognitive perspective. Unpublished Doctor of Philosophy in Technology Education. Linköping University, Norrköping.

    Google Scholar 

  • Björklund, L. (2008b). The repertory grid technique: making tacit knowledge explicit: assessing creative work and problem solving skills. In H. Middleton (Ed.), Researching technology education: Methods and techniques (pp. 46-69). Rotterdam, the Netherlands: Sense Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Björklund, L. (2009). The forming and assessment of creative skills, from a neurocognitive point of view. In A. Bekker, I. Mottier, & M. J. de Vries (Eds.), Strengthening the position of technology education in the curriculum: Proceedings of the PATT-22 Conference (pp. 64-76), Delft, the Netherlands, August 24-28, 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullier, J. (2001). Integrated model of visual processing. Brain Research Reviews, 36, 96-107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1999). Top-down attentional guidance based on implicit learning of visual covariation. Psychological Science, 10, 360-365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cowan, N. (2000). The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 87-114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cronin, M. A. (2004). A model of knowledge activation and insight in problem solving. Complexity, 9(5), 17-24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutujian, F. C. (1942). The rôle of identical stimulus-elements in judgments of similarity. The American Journal of Psychology, 55(1), 18-32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Day, S. B., & Goldstone, R. L. (2011). Analogical transfer from a simulated physical system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37(3), 551-567.

    Google Scholar 

  • Day, S. B., & Goldstone, R. L. (2012). The import of knowledge export: Connecting findings and theories of transfer of learning. Educational Psychologist, 47(3), 153-176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dijksterhuis, A. (2006). Where creativity resides: The generative power of unconscious thought. Consciousness and Cognition, 15, 135-146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. L., & Dreyfus, S. E. (1986). Mind over machine: The power of human intuition and expertise in the era of the computer. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, R. (2011). Jizz and the joy of pattern recognition: Virtuosity, discipline and the agency of insight in UK naturalists’ arts of seeing. Social Studies of Science, 41(6), 769-790.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engeström, Y., Engeström, R., & Kärkkäinen, M. (1995). Polycontextuality and boundary crossing in expert cognition: Learning and problem solving in complex work activities. Learning and Instruction, 5(4), 319-336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255-278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fallshore, M., & Schooler, J. W. (1995). Verbal vulnerability of perceptual expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(6), 1608-1623.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fleischman, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., Bienias, J. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2004). A longitudinal study of implicit and explicit memory in old persons. Psychology and Aging, 19(4), 617-625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hesketh, B. (1997). Dilemmas in training for transfer and retention. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 46(4), 317-386.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holland, J. G., & Henson, J. B. (1956). Transfer of training between quickened and unquickened tracking systems. Journal of Applied Psychology, 40(6), 362-366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulin, W. S., & Katz, D. (1934). Transfer of training in reading braille. The American Journal of Psychology, 46(4), 627-631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, G. (1924). The psychology of the gestalt. Journal of Educational Psychology, 15(7), 401-412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. H. (1908). The relation of special training and general intelligence. Educational Review, 36, 28-42.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2003). A perspective on judgment and choice: Mapping bounded rationality. American Psychologist, 58(9), 697-720.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, G. (2004). The power of intuition: How to use your gut feelings to make better decisions at work. New York: Currency/Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics and culture in everyday life. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, Y.-S., & Vakoch, D. A. (1996). Transfer and retention of implicit and explicit learning. British Journal of Psychology, 87(4), 637-652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehman, H. C. (1933). A class experiment in the transfer of training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 17(1), 77-82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126(1), 109-137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, M. D., Gaunt, R., Gilbert, D. T., & Trope, Y. (2002). Reflection and reflexion: A social cognitive neuroscience approach to attributional inference. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 34, 199-249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lincoln, R. S., & Smith, K. U. (1951). Transfer of training in tracking performance at different target speeds. Journal of Applied Psychology, 35(5), 358-362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindström, L. (2006). Creativity: What is it? Can you assess it? Can it be taught? Journal of Art and Design Education, 25(1).

    Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95, 492-527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (2002). An instance theory of attention and memory. Psychological Review, 109(2), 376-400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Low, D. R. (2006). Innovation and its interaction with market orientation: A study of Australian manufacturing smes. Sydney: University of Western Sydney.

    Google Scholar 

  • Macdonald, H. (2002). ‘What makes you a scientist is the way you look at things’: Ornithology and the observer 1930–1955. Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences, 33, 53-77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machin, M. A., & Fogarty, G. J. (1997). The effects of self-efficacy, motivation to transfer, and situational constraints on transfer intentions and transfer of training. Performance Improvement Quarterly, 10(2), 98-115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (2000). Priming of pop-out: III. A short-term implicit memory system beneficial for rapid target selection. Visual Cognition, 7(5), 571-595.

    Google Scholar 

  • Markman, A. B., Maddox, W. T., & Worthy, D. A. (2006). Choking and excelling under pressure. Psychological Science, 17(11), 944-948.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marois, R., & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(6), 296-305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marton, F. (2006). Sameness and difference in transfer. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 15(4), 499-535.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Masters, R. S. W. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology, 83, 343-358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Melcher, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (1996). The misremembrance of wines past: Verbal and perceptual expertise differentially mediate verbal overshadowing of taste memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 231-245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Middleton, H. (2002). Complex problem solving in a workplace setting. International Journal of Educational Research, 37, 67-84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (2008). Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia, 46(3), 774-785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, S. M. (1964). Tracking with a differential brightness display: I acquisition and transfer. Journal of Applied Psychology, 48(2), 115-122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moulaert, V., Verwijnen, M. G. M., Rikers, R., & Scherpbier, A. J. (2004). The effects of deliberate practice in undergraduate medical education. Medical Education, 38, 1044-1052.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G., Young, M., & Brooks, L. (2007). Non-analytical models of clinical reasoning: The role of experience. Medical Education, 41, 1140-1145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, G. R., & Brooks, L. R. (1997). The non-analytical basis of clinical reasoning. Advances in Health Sciences Education, 2, 173-184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nosofsky, R. M., & Zaki, S. R. (2002). Exemplar and prototype models revisited: Response strategies, selective attention, and stimulus generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28, 924-940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Polanyi, M. (1966). The tacit dimension. Gloucester, MA: Doubleday & Company, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poulton, E. C. (1969). Asymmetrical transfer in reading texts produces by teleprinter and by typewriter. Journal of Applied Psychology, 53(3), 244-249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reber, R., Ruch-Monachon, M.-A., & Perrig, W. J. (2007). Decomposing intuitive components in a conceptual problem solving task. Consciousness and Cognition, 16(2), 294-309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reeves, L. M., & Weisberg, R. W. (1994). The role of content and abstract information in analogical transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 115(3), 381-400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ritchie, M. L., & Michael, A. L. (1955). Transfer between instrument and contact flight training. Journal of Applied Psychology, 39(3), 145-149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ross, B. M. (1969). Sequential visual memory and the limited magic of the number seven. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 80(2), 339-347.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind (13th impr. ed. ed.). London: Hutchinson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R. (1989). On the course of forgetting in very long-term memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(2), 241-245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82, 171-177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stolpe, K., & Björklund, L. (2012a). Seeing the wood for the trees. Applying the dual memory system model to investigate expert teachers’ observational skills in natural ecological learning environments. International Journal of Science Education, 34(1), 101-125.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stolpe, K., & Björklund, L. (2012b). Students’ long-term memories from an ecology field excursion. Retelling a narrative as an interplay between implicit and explicit memories. Scandinavian Journal of Educational Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Chandler, P. (1991). Evidence for cognitive load theory. Cognition and Instruction, 8(4), 351-362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. G. W. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Phychologist Review, 10(3), 251-296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L. (1913). The psychology of learning (Vol. 2). New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thorndike, E. L., & Woodworth, R. S. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. II. The estimation of magnitudes. Psychological Review, 8(4), 384-395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thunholm, P. (2003). Decision making under time pressure: to evaluate or not to evaluate three options before the decision is made?

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunney, R. J. (2003). Implicit and explicit knowledge decay at different rates: A dissociation between priming and recognition in artificial grammar learning. Experimental Psychology, 50(2), 124-130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wallas, G. (1949). The art of thought. London: Watts & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodworth, R. S., & Thorndike, E. L. (1901). The influence of improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other functions: I. Psychological Review, 8(3), 247-261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Sense Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Björklund, L. (2013). Why Do They Not See What I See?. In: Middleton, H.E., Baartman, L.K.J. (eds) Transfer, Transitions and Transformations of Learning. International Technology Education Studies. SensePublishers, Rotterdam. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-6209-437-6_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics

Societies and partnerships