Skip to main content

Learning Progressions as Tools For Curriculum Development

Lessons from the Inquiry Project

  • Chapter
Learning Progressions in Science

Abstract

Cognitively-based curricula may take into account research on students’ difficulties with a particular topic (e.g., weight and density, inertia, the role of environment in natural selection) or domain-general learning principles (e.g., the importance of revisiting basic ideas across grades). Learning progressions (LPs) integrate and enrich those approaches by organizing students’ beliefs around core ideas in that domain, giving a rich characterization of what makes students’ initial ideas profoundly different from those of scientists, and specifying how to revisit those ideas within and across grades so that young children’s ideas can be progressively elaborated on and reconceptualized toward genuine scientific understanding.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 49.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Acher A, Arcà M. Children's representations in modeling scientific knowledge construction. In: Andersen C, Scheuer MN, Perez Echeveerria MP, Teubal E, editors. Representational systems and practices as learning tools in different fields of knowledge. Rotterdam, The Netherlands: Sense Publishers; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  • Au TK-F. Developing an intuitive understanding of substance kinds. Cognitive Psychology. 1994;27:71–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon R. The acquisition of physical knowledge in infancy: A summary in eight lessons. In: Goswami U, editor. Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development. London, UK: Blackwell Publishers; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey S. The origin of concepts. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carraher D, Schliemann A, Brizuela B, Earnest D. Arithmetic and algebra in early mathematics education. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education. 2006;37:87–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carraher D, Smith C, Wiser M, Schliemann A, Cayton-Hodges G. June). Paper presented at the Learning Progressions in Science (LeaPS) Conference, Iowa City, IA: Assessing students' evolving understandings about matter; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeBarger A, Ayala C, Minstrell J, Krauss P, Stanford T. April). Paper presented at the annual meeting of American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA: Facet-based progressions of student understanding of chemistry; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson DK. The development of material kind. Science Education. 1987;71:615–628.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driver R, Guesne E, Tiberghien A. Children's ideas in science. Milton Keynes, UK: Open University Press; 1985.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fauconnier G, Turner M. The way we think: Conceptual blending and the mind's hidden complexities. New York, NY: Basic Books; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hart B, Risley TR. The social world of children: Learning to talk. Baltimore, MD: P. Brooks Publishing; 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Imai, M.,& Mazuka, R. (2007). Revisiting language universals and linguistic relativity: Language relative construal of individuation constrained byuniversal ontology. Cognitive Science, 31, 385-413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson P. Progression in children's understanding of a "basic" particle theory: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Science Education. 1998;20:393–412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson P, Papageorgiou G. Rethinking the introduction of particle theory: A substance- based framework. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 2010;47:130–150.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn TS. The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press; 1962.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer R, Jaslow L, Curtis C. Developing understanding of measurement in elementary grades. In: Clements D, Bright G, editors. The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics: Yearbook on learning and teaching measurement. Reston, VA: The National Council of Teachers of Mathematics; 2003. p. 100–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer R, Pritchard C. Symbolizing space into being. In: Gravemeijer K, Lehrer R, van Oers B, Verschaffel L, editors. Symbolization, modeling and tool use in mathematics education. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press; 2002. p. 59–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer R, Schauble L. Developing model-based reasoning in mathematics and science. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology. 2000;21:39–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lehrer R, Schauble L, Strom D, Pligge M. Similarity of form and substance: Modeling material kind. In: Carver S, Klahr D, editors. Cognition and instruction: Twenty-five years of progress. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 2001. p. 39–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucy JA, Gaskins S. Interaction of language type and referent type in the development of nonverbal classification preferences. In: Gentner D, Goldin-Meadow S, editors. Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press; 2003. p. 465–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Margel H, Eylon B-S, Scherz Z. A longitudinal study of junior high school students' conceptions of the structure of materials. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 2008;45:132–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merritt J, Krajcik J. June). Paper presented at the Learning Progressions in Science (LeaPS) Conference, Iowa City, IA: Developing a calibrated progress variable for the particle nature of matter; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metz KE. Preschoolers' developing knowledge of the pan balance: From new representation to transformed problem solving. Cognition and Instruction. 1993;11:31–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nussbaum J. History and philosophy of science and the preparation for constructivist teaching: The case of particle theory. In: Mintzes J, Wandersee JH, Novak JD, editors. Teaching science for understanding. Boston, MA: Academic Press; 1997. p. 165–194.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget J, Inhelder B. The child's construction of quantities: Conservation and atomism. London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul; 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker S. The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature. New York, NY: Penguin Group; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schliemann, A., & Carraher, D. (2002). The evolution of mathematical understanding: Everyday vs. idealized reasoning. Developmental Review, 22, 242-266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shwartz Y, Weizman A, Fortus D, Krajcik J, Reiser B. The IQWST experience: Using coherence as a design principle for a middle school science curriculum. The Elementary School Journal. 2008;109:199–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith C, Carey S, Wiser M. On differentiation: A case study of the development of the concepts of size, weight and density. Cognition. 1985;21:177–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith C, Grosslight L, Davis H, Unger C. & Snir, J. Using conceptual models to teach inner city students about density: The promise and the prerequisites. Final report to the McDonnell Foundation; 1994.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith C, Maclin D, Grosslight L, Davis H. Teaching for understanding: A comparison of two approaches to teaching students about matter and density. Cognition and Instruction. 1997;15:317–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith C, Snir J, Grosslight L. Using conceptual models to facilitate conceptual change: The case of weight/density differentiation. Cognition and Instruction. 1992;9:221–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith CL, Wiser M, Anderson CW, Krajcik J. Implications of research on children's learning for standards and assessment: A proposed learning progression for matter and atomic- molecular theory. Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and Perspectives. 2006;4:1–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C., Wiser, M., & Carraher, D. (2010, March). Using a comparative, longitudinal study with upper elementary school students to test some assumptions of a learning progression for matter. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the National Association for Research in Science Teaching, Philadelphia, PA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snir J, Smith CL, Raz G. Linking phenomena with competing underlying models: A software tool for introducing students to the particulate model of matter. Science Education. 2003;87:794–830.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke ES. Core knowledge. American Psychologist. 2000;55:1233–1243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stavy R, Stachel D. Children's conceptions of changes in the state of matter: From solid to liquid. Archives de Psychologie. 1985;53:331–344.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens SY, Delgado C, Krajcik JS. Developing a hypothetical multidimensional learning progression for the nature of matter. Journal of Research in Science Teaching. 2010;47:687–715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Uzgiris I. Situational generality of conservation. Child Development. 1964;35:831–841.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vergnaud G. The theory of conceptual fields. In: Steffe LP, Nesher P, Cobb P, Goldin G, Greer B, editors. Theories of mathematical learning. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum; 1996. p. 219–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiser M, Citrin K, Drosos A, Hosek S. June). Poster presented at the annual Meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Quebec City, Canada: Young children's understanding of the relation between weight and material kind; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiser M, Fox V. November). Quantifying amount of material: A teaching intervention in pre-K and kindergarten. Paper presented at the International Science Education Symposium on the Particulate and Structural Concepts of Matter, University of Athens, Greece; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiser M, Smith CL. Teaching about matter in grades K-8: When should the atomic- molecular theory be introduced? In: Vosniadou S, editor. International handbook of research on conceptual change. New York, NY: Routledge; 2008. p. 205–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiser, M., & Smith, C. L. (2009, August). How does cognitive development inform the choice of core ideas in the physical sciences? Commissioned paper presented at the National Research Council Conference on Core Ideas in Science, Washington, DC. Retrieved from the National Academies website: http://www7.nationalacademies.org/bose/Wiser_Smith_CommissionedPaper.pdf

  • Wiser, M., Smith, C., Asbell-Clarke, J., & Doubler, S. (2009, April). Developing and refining a learning progression for matter: The inquiry project (grades 3-5). In C. Smith (Chair), Developing and refining a learning progression for matter from pre-K to grade 12: Commonalities and contrasts among four current projects. Symposium conducted at the annual meeting of American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Alicia C. Alonzo Amelia Wenk Gotwals

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Sense Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Wiser, M., Smith, C.L., Doubler, S. (2012). Learning Progressions as Tools For Curriculum Development. In: Alonzo, A.C., Gotwals, A.W. (eds) Learning Progressions in Science. SensePublishers, Rotterdam. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-6091-824-7_16

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics

Societies and partnerships