Skip to main content

On the Domain Specificity of Intervention Effects in Children’s Comprehension of Relative Clauses and Coordinate Clauses

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
New Trends in Language Acquisition Within the Generative Perspective

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 49))

  • 502 Accesses

Abstract

In this study, linear and hierarchical intervention effects are considered in children’s comprehension of coordinate clauses (CCs) and object relative clauses (ORCs), respectively. It is assumed that intervention effects due to crossing-dependency, generalised minimality or overextension of Relativized Minimality can be domain-specific. The aim of this study is to distinguish these effects from possible interference effects that are not exclusive to the language domain. The pattern of children’s errors of CCs in different tasks is analysed in relation to the pattern of errors of subject RCs, which can be ascribed to recency (in centre-embedding) or to difficulty in identifying the syntactic dependency at stake (in right-branching). Regarding ORCs, a memory task that simulates the demands of these sentences in a non-grammatical context was conducted with children at risk of language impairment in the syntactic domain (syn-LI) and a control group. The results of the analyses suggest the following: age differences and the task demands can favour the effect of domain-general or domain-specific factors; distinguishing grammatical dependencies (CC vs. RC) at the clause-boundary can be a problem in language development; and a small subgroup of children with symptoms of syn-LI is likely to have a memory-independent domain-specific difficulty.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    SLI—Specific Language Impairment. For terminology and criteria for the characterization of this syndrome, see Sect. 11.4.

  2. 2.

    The view that strategic behavior or a single analysis could account for across the board responses prevailed in the early studies of children’s comprehension of RCs (Sheldon 1974, 1977; de Villiers et al. 1979; Tavakolian 1981).

  3. 3.

    A pre-test was conducted in order to evaluate children’s ability to cope with the task. 62% of the 5 year-olds and the totality of the 7-year-olds demonstrated an ability to deal with the test-sentences as linguistic objects.

  4. 4.

    The experiment conducted in Hebrew included CCs and RCs, while that in EP dealt with CCs with intransitive verbs only due to the ambiguity of who-questions (subject or object) with transitive verbs in EP. Who-questions with transitive verbs are not ambiguous in BP.

  5. 5.

    The prominence of an element in a c-command position may reflect syntax-independent processing factors affecting sentence encoding from the point of view of the speaker, insofar as headedness can be viewed as an epiphenomenal by-product of thematic assignment and minimal search (Chomsky 2009).

  6. 6.

    These data were collected in the context of preliminary research directed towards the creation of a battery of tests for the screening of language impairment in BP-speaking children. The comprehension abilities of a total of 205 children from 3 to 7 years were assessed. The sentences included passives, coordinate clauses, coordination in the DP, Wh-questions, RCs, reflexives, and pronominals.

  7. 7.

    The whole procedure was also sensitive to the different demands of each task. The Secret Number Task was the most difficult one. The difference between the mean target responses in this task and in the backward digit recall task (executive control) was highly significant: F(1,36) = 380, p < .000001, SS = 183.08, MSe = 0.48, showing that the former was particularly hard and displayed considerable individual variance.

  8. 8.

    Cf. overlapping areas for verbal WM and language in Fedorenko et al. (2011, 2012).

References

  • Adani, F. (2011). Rethinking the acquisition of relative clauses in Italian: Towards a grammatically based account. Journal of Child Language, 38(1), 141–165. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000909990250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adani, F. (2012). Some notes on the acquisition of relative clauses: New data and open questions. Internet celebration for Luigi Rizzi’s 60th birthday, CISCL, Siena (pp. 6–13). http://www.ciscl.unisi.it/gg60/papers/adani.pdf.

  • Adani, F., van der Lely, H. K. J., Forgiarini, M., & Guasti, M. T. (2010). Grammatical feature dissimilarities make relative clauses easier: A comprehension study with Italian children. Lingua, 120(9), 2148–2166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.03.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Archibald, L. M. D., & Gathercole, S. E. (2006). Short-term and working memory in specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 41(6), 675–693. https://doi.org/10.1080/13682820500442602.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arosio, F., Guasti, M. T., Molteni, B., Panzeri, F., & Magazù, S. (2017). The comprehension of Italian relative clauses in poor readers and in children with specific language impairment. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 2(1). https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.107.

  • Augusto, M. R. A., Corrêa, L. M. S., & Forster, R. (2012). An argument for DPs as phases in an integrated model of on-line computation: The immediate mapping of complex DPs with relative clauses. ReVEL, Special Issue, 6, 7–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (1990). Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Befi-Lopes, D. M., & Toba, J. R. (2012). How do children and adolescents with specific language impairment comprehend verbal information? Revista da Sociedade Brasileira de Fonoaudiologia, 17(1), 106–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G. (1970). The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In J. R. Hayes (Ed.), Cognition and the development of language (pp. 279–362). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bidelman, G. M., & Walker, B. S. (2017). Attentional modulation and domain-specificity underlying the neural organization of auditory categorical perception. European Journal of Neuroscience, 45(5), 690–699. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.13526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2000). Pragmatic language impairment: A correlate of SLI, a distinct subgroup, or part of the autistic continuum? In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children: Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 99–113). New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2006). What causes specific language impairment in children? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(5), 217–221. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00439.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2017). Why is it so hard to reach agreement on terminology? The case of developmental language disorder (DLD). International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 52(6), 671–680. https://doi.org/10.1111/1460-6984.12335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., Snowling, M. J., Thompson, P. A., & Greenhalgh, T. (2016). CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary delphi consensus study. Identifying language impairments in children. PLOS ONE, 11(7). e0158753. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C. (2014). The roots of current biolinguistic thought: Revisiting the ‘Chomsky-Piaget debate’ in the context of the revival of Biolinguistics. Teorema, 33(1), 83–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Botting, N., & Marshall, C. (2017). Domain-specific and domain-general approaches to developmental disorders. In L. C. Centifanti & D. M. Williams (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of developmental psychopathology (1st ed., pp. 139–159). Hoboken, New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons Ltd. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118554470.ch6.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Burton, S., & Grimshaw, J. (1992). Coordianation and VP-internal subjects. Linguistic Inquiry, 23(2), 305–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carey, S., & Spelke, E. (1994). Domain-specific knowledge and conceptual change. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 169–200). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511752902.008.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of B. F. Skinner’s verbal behavior. Language, 35(1), 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on government and binding. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin and use. New York: PraegerScientific.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2001). Beyond explanatory adequacy. MIT Occasional Papers in Linguistics, 20(May), 1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2005). Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry, 36(1), 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1162/0024389052993655.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2009). Opening remarks. In M. Piatelli-Palmarini, J. Uriagereka, & P. Salaburu (Eds.), Of minds and language: A dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque Country (pp. 13–43). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2013). Problems of projection. Lingua, 130, 33–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2012.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A. A. (2003). A family aggregation study: The influence of family history and other risk factors on language development. Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research, 46, 261–72. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2003/021).

  • Colaço, M. (2005). Configurações de coordenação aditiva: Tipologia, concordância e extracção. Universidade de Lisboa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Contemori, C., & Garraffa, M. (2010). Comparison of modalities in SLI syntax: A study on the comprehension and production of non-canonical sentences. Lingua, 120(8), 1940–1955. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.02.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G., Botting, N., & Faragher, B. (2001). Psycholinguistic markers for specific language impairment (SLI). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(6), 741–748. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corrêa, L. M. S. (1986). On the comprehension of relative clauses: A developmental study with reference to Portuguese. London: University of London.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corrêa, L. M. S. (1995a). An alternative assessment of children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 24(3), 183–203.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corrêa, L. M. S. (1995b). The relative difficulty of children’s comprehension of relative clauses: A procedural account. In K. E. Nelson & Z. Réger (Eds.), Children’s language (Vol. 8, pp. 225–244). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corrêa, L. M. S. (2012). O DEL à luz de hipóteses psico/linguísticas: Avaliação de habilidades linguísticas e implicações para uma possível intervenção em problemas de linguagem de natureza sintática. Veredas Online, Special Issue 207–236.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corrêa, L. M. S. (2014). Interface information and computational cost: An integrated procedural approach to language acquisition with some implications for SLI. In A. Lúcia Santos, J. Costa, A. Fiéis, M. João Freitas, & M. Lobo (Eds.), New directions in the acquisition of Romance languages: Selected proceedings of The Romance Turn V (pp. 2–34) New Castle-upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corrêa, L. M. S., & Augusto, M. R. A. (2011). Possible loci of SLI from a both linguistic and psycholinguistic perspective. Lingua, 121(3). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corrêa, L. M. S., Teixeira, E. N., & Rodrigues, E. d. S. (2017). The use of eye tracker in the discrimination of linguistic and image processing demands in a picture-identification task. Paper presented in ECEM: 19th European Conference on Eye-Movements (Aug 20th–24th). Wuppertal.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Villiers, J. G., Hakuta, K., & Cohen, M. (1979). Children’s comprehension of relative clauses. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 8(5), 499–518. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01067332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diessel, H., & Tomasello, M. (2000). The development of relative clauses in spontaneous child speech. Cognitive Linguistics, 11(1/2), 131–51. https://doi.org/0936±5907/00/0011±0131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downing, P. E., Chan, A. W. Y., Peelen, M. V., Dodds, C. M., & Kanwisher, N. (2006). Domain specificity in visual cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 16(10), 1453–1461. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhj086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fedorenko, E., Behr, M. K., & Kanwisher, N. (2011). Functional specificity for high-level linguistic processing in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(39), 16428–16433. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1112937108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fedorenko, E., Duncan, J., & Kanwisher, N. (2012). Language-selective and domain-general regions lie side by side within Broca’s area. Current Biology, 22(21), 2059–2062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.09.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fedorenko, E., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2014). Reworking the language network. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(3), 120–126. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Forster, R., & Corrêa, L. M. S. (2017). On the asymmetry between subject and object relative clauses in discourse context. Revista de Estudos da Linguagem, 25(3). https://doi.org/10.17851/2237-2083.25.3.1225-1254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Franck, J., Frauenfelder, U., & Rizzi, L. (2007). A syntactic analysis of interference in subject-verb agreement. In V. S. Schütze & C. T. Ferreira (Eds.), The state of the art in speech error research: Proceedings of the LSA Institute Workshop. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics (Vol. 53, pp. 173–90).

    Google Scholar 

  • Franck, J., Lassi, G., Frauenfelder, U., & Rizzi, L. (2006). Agreement and movement: A syntactic analysis of attraction. Cognition, 101(1), 173–216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.10.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedmann, N., Belletti, A., & Rizzi, L. (2009). Relativized relatives: Types of intervention in the acquisition of A-bar dependencies. Lingua, 119(1), 67–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2008.09.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedmann, N., & Costa, J. (2010). The child heard a coordinated sentence and wondered: On children’s difficulty in understanding coordination and relative clauses with crossing dependencies. Lingua, 120(6), 1502–1515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2009.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friedmann, N., & Novogrodsky, R. (2008). Subtypes of SLI: SYSLI, PHOSLI, LESLI, and PRASLI. In A. Gavarró & M. J. Freitas (Eds.), Language Acquisition and Development (pp. 205–2017). Newcastle-Upon-Tyne: Cambridge Scholars Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garraffa, M., & Grillo, N. (2008). Canonicity effects as grammatical phenomena. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 21(2), 177–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2007.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, R. (2000). Domain specificity in cognitive development: Universals and nonuniversals. Child Development, 71(4), 854–856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghandour, H., Eldin, S. K., Sallam, Y., & Mahmoud, S. (2018). Associated comorbidities of specific language impairment. Benha Medical Journal, 35(2), 115–121. https://doi.org/10.4103/bmfj.bmfj_155_17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodluck, H., & Tavakolian, S. (1982). Competence and processing in children’s grammar of relative clauses. Cognition, 11(1), 1–27. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(82)90002-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2001). Memory interference during language processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(6), 1411–1423. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.27.6.1411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Johnson, M. (2004). Effects of noun phrase type on sentence complexity. Journal of Memory and Language, 51(1), 97–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.02.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, P. C., & Lowder, M. W. (2012). Complex sentence processing: A review of theoretical perspectives on the comprehension of relative clauses. Language and Linguistics Compass, 6(7), 403–415.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grillo, N. (2009). Generalized minimality: Feature impoverishment and comprehension deficits in agrammatism. Lingua, 119(10), 1426–1443. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2008.04.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grodzinsky, Y. (1989). Agrammatic comprehension of relative clause. Brain and Language, 37(3), 480–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Tecumseh Fitch, W. (2010). The faculty of language: What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? In R. K. Larson, V. Depréz, & H. Yamakido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 14–42). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Hirschfeld, L. A., & Gelman, S. A. (1994). Toward a topography of mind: An introduction to domain specificity. In L. A. Hirschfeld & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind (pp. 3–36). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511752902.002.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Jakubowicz, C. (2003). Computational complexity and the acquisition of functional categories by French-speaking children with SLI. Linguistics, 41(2), 175–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakubowicz, C. (2011). Measuring derivational complexity: New evidence from typically developing and SLI learners of L1 French. Lingua, 121(3), 339–351. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jakubowicz, C. (2018). Hipóteses psicolinguísticas sobre a natureza do déficit específico da linguagem (DEL). In L. M. S. Corrêa (Ed.), Aquisição da linguagem e problemas do desenvolvimento linguistico (2nd ed., pp. 265–346). Rio de Janeiro: Ed. PUC-Rio.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kanwisher, N. (2010). Functional specificity in the human brain: A window into the functional architecture of the mind. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(25), 11163–11170. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1005062107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, R. S. (1994). The antisymmetry of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (1994). The birth and nurturance of concepts by domains: The origin of the concept of living things. In S. A. Hirschfeld & L. A. Gelman (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 234–254). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Keil, F. C. (2007). Biology and beyond: Domain specificity in a broader developmental context. Human Development, 50(1), 31–38. https://doi.org/10.1159/000097682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kidd, E., & Bavin, E. L. (2002). English-speaking children’s comprehension of relative clauses: Evidence for constraints on development. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 31(6), 599–617.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leonard, L. B. (2014). Children with specific language impairment and their contribution to the study of language development. Journal of Child Language, 41(1), 38–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000914000130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maillart, C., Schelstraete, M.-A., & Hupet, M. (2004). Phonological representations in children with specific language impairment. Enfance, 56(1), 187–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marinis, T. (2011). On the nature and cause of specific language impairment: A view from sentence processing and infant research. Lingua, 121(3), 463–475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNally, L. (1992). VP coordination and the VP-internal subject hypothesis. Linguistic Inquiry, 23(2), 336–341.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehler, J., Christophe, A., & Ramus, F. (2000). What we know about the initial state for language. Image, language, brain: Papers from the first mind-brain articulation project symposium, 33(1), 51–75. http://www.lscp.net/persons/ramus/docs/mehler00.pdf.

  • Montgomery, J. W. (2003). Working memory and comprehension in children with specific language impairment: What we know so far. Journal of Communication Disorders, 36(3), 221–231. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0021-9924(03)00021-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Montgomery, J. W., Magimairaj, B. M., & Finney, M. C. (2010). Working memory and specific language impairment: An update on the relation and perspectives on assessment and treatment. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 19(1), 78–94. https://doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2009/09-0028).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mueller, K. L. (2012). Causation, correlation, or confound? What the comorbidity of language impairment and ADHD can tell us about the etiology of these disorders. ProQuest Dissertations and Theses. University of Iowa.

    Google Scholar 

  • Newport, E. (1990). Maturational constraints on language learning. Cognitive Science, 14, 11–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Novogrodsky, R. (2007). Deficit in the syntactic chain: The characteristics of the syntactic impairment and modularity in SLI. Tel Aviv: Tel Aviv University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Novogrodsky, R., & Friedmann, N. (2006). The production of relative clauses in syntactic SLI: A window to the nature of the impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 8(4), 364–375. https://doi.org/10.1080/14417040600919496.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, J. (2001). Sideward movement. Linguistic Inquiry, 31(2), 303–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Oetting, J., & Rice, M. L. (1994). Plural acquisition in children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36(6), 1236–1248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ortega-Santos, I. (2011). On relativized minimality, memory and cue-based parsing. Iberia—An International Journal of Theoretical Linguistics, 3(1), 35–64.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (1980). Language and learning: The debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (1994). Ever since language and learning: Afterthoughts on the Piaget-Chomsky debate. Cognition, 50(1–3), 315–346. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(94)90034-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, S., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Working memory test battery for children (WMTB-C). Harlow: Pearspn.

    Google Scholar 

  • Poeppel, D. (2011). Genetics and language: A neurobiological perspective on the missing link (-ing hypotheses). Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 3(4), 381–387. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11689-011-9097-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poll, G. H., Betz, S. K., & Miller, C. A. (2010). Identification of clinical markers of specific language impairment in adults. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53(2), 414–429. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2009/08-0016).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I., & Allen, D. A. (1983). Developmental language disorders: Nosologic considerations. In U. Kirk (Ed.), Neuropsychology of language, reading and spelling (pp. 155–184). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L., & Ratner, N. B. (1996). Phonetic profiles of toddlers with specific expressive language impairment (SLI-E). Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(1), 153–165. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3901.153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 39(6), 1239–1257. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3906.1239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, L. (1990). Relativized minimality (Linguistic Inquiry Monographs 16). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, L. (2004). Locality and left periphery. In A. Belletti (Ed.), Structure and beyond. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, L. (2013). Locality. Lingua, 130, 169–186. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2012.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosa, H. R., Pires, M. L. N., Alves, I. C. B., & Esteves, C. (2013). Standardization study of the R-2: Non verbal intelligence test for children. Boletim da Academia Paulista de Psicologia, 33(85), 373–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schulz, P., & Roeper, T. (2011). Acquisition of exhaustivity in wh-questions: A semantic dimension of SLI? Lingua, 121(3), 383–407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, A. (1974). The role of parallel functions in the acquisition of relative clauses in English. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 231–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sheldon, A. (1977). On strategies for processing relative clauses: A comparison of children and adults. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 6(4), 305–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S. (2000). Core knowledge. American Psychology, 55(11), 1233–1243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2017). A new look at domain specificity: Insights from social neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(9), 559–567. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stavrakaki, S. (2001). Comprehension of reversible relative clauses in specifically language impaired and normally developing Greek children. Brain and Language, 77(3), 419–431. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2000.2412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tavakolian, S. (1981). The conjoined-clause analysis of relative clauses. In S. L. Tavakolian (Ed.), Language acquisition and linguistic theory (pp. 167–187). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., Lum, J. A. G., & Conti-Ramsden, G. (2014). Domain specificity. In V. Brooks & P. J. Kempe (Eds.), Encyclopedia of language development (pp. 163–166). Sage Publications. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1561-1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, M. T., & Pierpont, E. I. (2005). Specific language impairment is not specific to language: The procedural deficit hypothesis. Cortex, 41(3), 399–433. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70276-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J. A., & Lewis, R. L. (2003). Distinguishing effects of structure and decay on attachment and repair: A cue-based parsing account of recovery from misanalyzed ambiguities. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(3), 285–316. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0749-596X(03)00081-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dyke, J. A., & McElree, B. (2006). Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 157–166. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Lely, H. K. J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: Insight from grammatical-SLI. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 53–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vasishth, S., & Lewis, R. L. (2005). An activation-based model of sentence processing as skilled memory retrieval. Cognitive Science, 29, 375–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vercelli, D. (2009). Language in an epigenetic framework. In M. Piatelli-Palmarini, J. Uriagereka, & P. Salaburu (Eds.), Of minds and language: A dialogue with Noam Chomsky in the Basque Country (pp. 97–107). Orlando: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wanner, E., & Maratsos, M. (1978). An ATN approach to comprehension. In M. Halle, J. Bresnan, & G. A. Miller (Eds.), Linguistic theory and psychological reality (pp. 119–161). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warren, T., & Gibson, E. (2002). The influence of referential processing on sentence complexity. Cognition, 85(1), 79–112. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12086714.

  • Williams, D. M., & Lind, S. E. (2012). Comorbidity and diagnosis of developmental disorders. In C. Marshall (Ed.), Current issues in developmental disorders (pp. 19–45). London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203100288.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I acknowledge the contribution of Luiza Frizzo Trugo, a former student, now in a different career, for her ideas on the elaboration of the video-game style material of the Secret Number Task and for her dedicated work in the collection and preliminary analysis of the data obtained in this task. The preliminary analysis of the results was presented at the 13th IASCL Conference (International Association for the Study of Child Language) 2014, Amsterdam, as Corrêa, L. M. S., & Trugo, L. F., ‘Feature interference and WM in the comprehension of object relative clauses by language impaired children’. I also acknowledge the support of FAPERJ (Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado do Rio de Janeiro) for the study of typical and atypical language development and for CNPq (Brazilian National Research Council) for the theoretical research and composition of this chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Letícia Maria Sicuro Corrêa .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature B.V.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Corrêa, L.M.S. (2020). On the Domain Specificity of Intervention Effects in Children’s Comprehension of Relative Clauses and Coordinate Clauses. In: Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Suárez-Gómez, C. (eds) New Trends in Language Acquisition Within the Generative Perspective. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 49. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-024-1932-0_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-024-1932-0_11

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-024-1931-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-024-1932-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics