Skip to main content

One Feature—One Head: Features as Functional Heads in Language Acquisition and Attrition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 49))

Abstract

This chapter explores the ramifications of envisaging an architecture of syntax in which each functional projection (head) consists of only one singleton feature. Following Kayne, (2005), I refer to this as the One Feature—One Head (1F1H) architecture. Beyond providing a sketch of the core desiderata associated with this architecture, I discuss how L1 and L2 acquisition would take place within the 1F1H-architecture, demonstrating that analyses undertaken in the Feature Reassembly-model can be directly subsumed into this architecture in a straightforward and conceptually appealing way. Finally, I turn my attention to language attrition, addressing how the 1F1H-architecture adopts a version of Scontras et al.’s, (2018) call for representational economy in the grammatical representations of languages affected by attrition (see also Putnam et al., 2019).

This manuscript has benefited from comments and questions during GALA 13 and other discussions as well as thoughtful comments and questions from two anonymous reviewers. I am especially grateful to Antonio Fábregas, Terje Lohndal, Jason Rothman, Liliana Sánchez, and Tom Stroik for comments and criticisms on an earlier version. Finally, I would like to thank Robert Klosinski for his editing assistance. All remaining shortcomings are the fault of the author.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   119.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   159.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Rizzi’s extended CP has since been expanded to many other languages, such as German (see, e.g., te Velde 2017).

  2. 2.

    For ease of exposition, we will continue to label the branching projection associated with these features as specifiers, while acknowledging that this terminology and the function of these structural units are a matter of debate (see, e.g., Starke 2009, 2011; Lohndal 2014).

  3. 3.

    For earlier attempts establishing a tripartite division of label of functional domains in the syntax, see Platzack (2000), Grohmann (2003) and Putnam (2007).

  4. 4.

    Some exo-skeletal approaches to event decomposition adopt the approach that lexical elements void of argument structure and some phonological information are inserted into the syntax as category-neutral √roots. To establish their categorical status, these √roots are introduced by a light functional head (e.g., v, n, etc.). Establishing the exact properties of √roots is a matter of ongoing debate (see e.g., Alexiadou et al. 2014). Here we attempt to make categorial distinctions post-syntactic rather than relying on light heads. See also Borer (2013) for an exo-skeletal approach that does not postulate light functional heads to introduce √roots.

  5. 5.

    The role of Merge in structure building takes center stage in this debate. Is Merge understood to be free (Boeckx 2010, 2014) or is it driven by some interpretive, preferably feature-driven component (Stroik 2009)? Although we leave this debate for future concern, the structural design of language proposed by Stroik and Putnam (2013) and its suggestion that interpretable features projected throughout syntactic structure can comply with nanosyntactic desiderata. To achieve this, the model would have to abandon certain assumptions about the lexicon (in Chap. 2), but aside from this issue, their model would provide a derivational account of the fseq suggested by Ramchand and Svenonius (2014) and Wiltschko (2014) that is a discrete combinatorial system rather than a recursive-embedded one (see Nordström 2017, 2018).

  6. 6.

    The notion of phrasal movement resembles Stroik’s (1999) Survive Principle to the extent that both operations motivate syntactic movement, or ‘repulsion’ in this case, by the need to optimize Full Interpretation.

  7. 7.

    Here we take Inf to represent the subcomponents of event structure proposed in Ramchand (2008).

  8. 8.

    An anonymous reviewer raises the question as to whether this hierarchy can be understood as a continuum between syntactic structure and morphology. Such an analogy cannot be established, because each designation in Biberauer and Roberts’s (2017) hierarchy refers to the range of a class functional heads that share a particular feature or attribute, with nanoparameters referring to the smallest subset of functional heads.

  9. 9.

    The generation of mental representations, including linguistic representations, is gradient to varying degrees. The production of S- and L-trees and as well as their interface is best understood as probabilistic in nature. The architecture and structures described here are compatible with Bod’s (2009) probabilistic tree structures.

  10. 10.

    Putnam (2017) makes the case for feature-valuation via constraint satisfaction and illustrates how this can be achieved in LFG. Although this is traditionally understood as a lexicalist framework, the appeal of a distributed mapping system of features and attributes is similar in some respects to the interpretation of lexical entries in Nanosyntax. A fundamental difference between Putnam (2017) and this exposition is in the nature of syntactic projections.

  11. 11.

    The situation becomes a bit more complicated with the source grammars of a bilingual differ on parameters such as the morphophonology of the expression of particular features, whether or not a particular feature is covert (as in the sense of Cho and Slabakova (2014) discussed above, or if word order difference between the two exist. Goldrick et al. (2016) present a probabilistic, constraint-based account of code-switching that could be integrated into the 1F1H-architecture. I leave this for future research considerations.

References

  • Adger, D. (2006). Combinatorial variability. Journal of Linguistics, 42, 503–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adger, D. (2013). A syntax of substance. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adger, D., & Svenonius, P. (2011). Features in minimalist syntax. In C. Boeckx (Ed.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic minimalism (pp. 27–51). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexiadou, A., Borer, H., & Schäfer, F. (Eds.). (2014). The syntax of roots and the roots of syntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arche, M. J. (2006). Individuals in time: Tense, aspect and the individual/stage distinction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baker, M. C. (2003). Lexical categories: Verbs, nouns, and adjectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Baunaz, L., Haegeman, L., De Clerq, K., & Lander, E. (Eds.). (2018). Exploring nanosyntax. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biberauer, T., & Roberts, I. (2017). Parameter setting. In A. Ledgeway & I. Roberts (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of historical syntax (pp. 134–162). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Blaszczak, J., Giannakidou, A., Klimek-Jankowska, D., & Migdalski, K. (Eds.). (2016). Mood, aspect, modality revisited: New answers to old questions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bobaljik, J. D., & Thráinsson, H. (1998). Two heads aren’t always better than one. Syntax, 1, 37–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bod, R. (2009). From exemplar to grammar: A probabilistic analogy-based model of language learning. Cognitive Science, 33, 752–793.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C. (2010). A tale of two minimalisms: Reflections on the plausibility of crash-proof syntax, and its free-merge alternative. In M. T. Putnam (Ed.), Exploring crash-proof grammars (pp. 105–124). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C. (2014). Elementary syntactic structures: Prospects of a feature-free syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Boeckx, C. (2016). Considerations pertaining to the nature of logodiversity. In E. Luis, O. Fernández-Soriano, & A. Mendikoetxea (Eds.), Rethinking parameters (pp. 64–104). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (2005). Structuring sense: The normal course of events (Vol. 2). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Borer, H. (2013). Structuring sense: Taking form (Vol. 3). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D., & Chumakina, M. (2013). What there might be and what there is: An introduction to Canonical Typology. In D. Brown, M. Chumakina, & G. G. Corbett (Eds.), Canonical morphology and syntax (pp. 1–19). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cho, J., & Slabakova, R. (2014). Interpreting definiteness in a second language without articles: The case of L2 Russian. Second Language Research, 30(2), 159–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (2005). Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry, 36, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, G. (1999). Adverbs and functional heads: A cross-linguistic perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cinque, G., & Rizzi, L. (2010). The cartography of syntactic structures. In B. Heine & H. Narrog (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic analysis (pp. 51–65). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corbett, G. G. (2008). Features and universals. In S. Scalise, E. Magni, & A. Bisetto (Eds.), Universals of language today (pp. 129–143). Heidelberg: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, Ö. (1985). Tense and aspect systems. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dékány, É. (2009). The nanosyntax of Hungarian postpositions. Nordlyd, 36, 41–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eguren, L., Fernández-Soriano, O., & Mendikoetxea, A. (Eds.). (2016). Rethinking parameters. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Embick, D., & Noyer, R. (2007). Distributed morphology and the syntax/morphology interface. In G. Ramchand & C. Reiss (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of linguistic interfaces (pp. 289–324). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldrick, M., Putnam, M. T., & Schwarz, L. (2016). Co-activation in bilingual grammars: A computational account of code mixing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(5), 857–876.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grohmann, K. K. (2003). Prolific domains: On the anti-locality of movement dependencies. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haegeman, L., & Lander, E. (2018). Nanonsyntax: The basics. In L. Baunaz, L. Haegeman, K. De Clerq, & E. Lander (Eds.), Exploring nanosyntax (pp. 3–56). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Julien, M. (2002). Syntax heads and word formation. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kayne, R. (2005). Movement and silence. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kratzer, A. (1996). Severing the external argument from the verb. In J. Rooryck & L. Zaring (Eds.), Phrase structure and the lexicon (pp. 109–137). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Lardiere, D. (1998). Dissociating syntax from morphology in a divergent L2 end-state grammar. Second Language Research, 14(4), 359–375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lardiere, D. (2008). Feature-assembly in second language acquisition. In J. M. Liceras, H. Zobl, & H. Goodluck (Eds.), The role of formal features in second language acquisition (pp. 107–140). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lardiere, D. (2009). Some thoughts on the contrastive analysis of features in second language acquisition. Second Language Research, 25(2), 173–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leu, T. (2015). The architecture of determiners. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lightfoot, D. (2006). How new languages emerge. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lohndal, T. (2014). Phrase structure and argument structure: A case-study of the syntax-semantics interface. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mateu, J. F., Fábregas, A., & Putnam, M. (Eds.). (2015). Contemporary linguistic parameters. London: Bloomsbury.

    Google Scholar 

  • Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Montrul, S. (2015). The acquisition of heritage languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nie, Y. (2015a). French morphology and the pieces of verbal inflection. MA Thesis, University of Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nie, Y. (2015b). Tense and modality in French verbal morphology. In S. Vinerte (Ed.), Proceedings of CLA 2015 (pp. 1–15). http://cla-acl.ca/wp-content/uploads/Nie-2015.pdf. Accessed April 25, 2019.

  • Nordström, J. (2017). Language without narrow syntax. The Linguistic Review, 34(4), 687–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nordström, J. (2018). Language as sound and meaning. Habilitation Thesis, Johan Wolfgang Goethe-Universität-Frankfurt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, T. (1990). Events in the semantics of English. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pietroski, P. M. (2007). Systematicity via monadicity. Croatian Journal of Philosophy, 7, 343–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Platzack, C. (2000). Multiple interfaces. In Cognitive interfaces: Constraints on linking cognitive information, Emile van der Zee, and Urpo (pp. 21–53). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Polinsky, M. (2018). Heritage languages and their speakers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, M. (2007). Scrambling and the survive principle. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, M. (2017). Feature reassembly as constraint satisfaction. The Linguistic Review, 34(3), 533–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, M. (2019). The (in)stability of grammars. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 41, 275–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, M., & Sánchez, L. (2013). What’s so incomplete about incomplete acquisition?—A prolegomenon to modeling heritage grammars. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 3(4), 478–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, M., Carlson, M., & Reitter, D. (2018). Integrated, not isolated: Defining typological proxy-mity in an integrated multilingual architecture. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2212. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02212.

  • Putnam, M., Perez-Cortés, S., & Sánchez, L. (2019). Language attrition and the feature reassembly hypothesis. In M. S. Schmid & B. Köpke (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of language attrition (pp. 18–24). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pylkkänen, L. (2008). Introducing arguments. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ramchand, G. (2008). Verb meaning and the lexicon: A first-phase syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ramchand, G., & Svenonius, P. (2014). Deriving the functional hierarchy. Language Sciences, 46, 152–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzi, L. (1997). The fine structure of the left periphery. In L. Haegeman (Ed.), Elements of grammar (pp. 281–337). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Scontras, G., Polinsky, M., & Fuchs, Z. (2018). In support of representational economy: Agreement in heritage Spanish. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 3(1), 1. http://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schein, B. (1993). Plurals and events. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmid, M. S. (2011). Language attrition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Shlonsky, U. (2010). The cartographic enterprise in syntax. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4(6), 417–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slabakova, R. (2009). Features or parameters: Which one makes SLA easier and more interesting to study? Second Language Research, 25(2), 313–324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slabakova, R. (2015). Acquiring temporal meanings without tense morphology: The case of L2 Mandarin Chinese. The Modern Language Journal, 99(2), 283–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sorace, A. (2011). Pinning down the concept of “interface” in bilingualism. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism, 1(1), 1–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Starke, M. (2009). Nanosyntax: A short primer to a new approach to language. Nordlyd, 36(1), 1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starke, M. (2011). Towards an election solution of language variation. Unpublished manuscript, University of Tromsø (Available at LingBuzz/001183).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroik, T. (1999). The survive principle. Linguistic Analysis, 29, 278–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stroik, T. (2009). Locality in minimalist syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Stroik, T., & Putnam, M. (2013). The structural design of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • te Velde, J. R. (2017). German V2 and the PF-interface: Evidence from dialects. Journal of Germanic Linguistics, 29(2), 147–194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ticio, M. E. (2005). Locality and anti-locality in Spanish DPs. Syntax, 8(3), 229–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Hout, A. (1996). Event semantics of verb frame alternations. PhD dissertation, Tilburg University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westergaard, M. (2013). The acquisition of linguistic variation: parameters vs. micro-cues. In T. Lohndal (Ed.), In search of universal grammar: From Old Norse to Zoque (pp. 275–298). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westergaard, M. (2014). Linguistic variation and micro-cues in first language acquisition. Linguistic Variation, 14(1), 26–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wiltschko, M. (2014). The universal structure of categories: Towards a formal typology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Zeijlstra, H. (2008). On the syntactic flexibility of formal features. In T. Biberauer (Ed.), The limits of syntactic variation (pp. 143–174). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael T. Putnam .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2020 Springer Nature B.V.

About this chapter

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this chapter

Putnam, M.T. (2020). One Feature—One Head: Features as Functional Heads in Language Acquisition and Attrition. In: Guijarro-Fuentes, P., Suárez-Gómez, C. (eds) New Trends in Language Acquisition Within the Generative Perspective. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 49. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-024-1932-0_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-024-1932-0_1

  • Published:

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-024-1931-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-024-1932-0

  • eBook Packages: Social SciencesSocial Sciences (R0)

Publish with us

Policies and ethics