Skip to main content

The Brain and Its Maps: An Illustrative History

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Mapping Across Academia

Abstract

The human brain has been studied and mapped in a number of ways since antiquity. This chapter reviews investigative methods and the evolution of human brain maps from ancient times to the present. Special focus is given to the shift in modern history from purely structural mapping, through purely functional mapping, to neuropsychological maps displaying the relation between structure and function. The historical review that begins the chapter presents a number of key findings and investigators, major historical debates and detours in the field, and the more recently developed synergies between clinical treatment, neuropsychological measurement, and technological advancement. An introduction to current brain mapping techniques presents the mechanisms, strengths, and weaknesses of X-ray computed tomography (CT), single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), positron emission tomography (PET), electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial electrical stimulation (TES), and the various forms of magnetic resonance imaging (MRI). A final section reviews promising current approaches and initiatives in human brain mapping and the value they may add to neuropsychological investigation and the refinement of human brain maps. These relatively new approaches include more advanced statistical analyses, better data management and informatics specialized to handle very large data sets, functional network analysis, nonlinear and chaos/complexity analysis, development of whole-brain connectome maps, and new advances in the imaging of the microstructure of brain cells. This chapter is organized in such a way as to highlight the shifts in preferred metaphors guiding different eras of human brain research–from inert lump of flesh, to a container for mental and spiritual functions, to mechanistic pump, to an information processing computer , to a hyper-connected and mutable network.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 139.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 179.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Barr, W. (2008). The historical development of the neuropsychological test battery. In J. Morgan & J. Ricker (Eds.), Textbook of clinical neuropsychology. New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blonder, L. X., Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). The role of the right hemisphere in emotional communication. Brain, 114(3), 1115–1127.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Broadbent, D. E. (1957). A mechanical model for human attention and immediate memory. Psychological Review, 3(64), 205–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, E., & Dewhurst, K. (1996). An illustrated history of brain function: Imaging the brain from antiquity to the present. Novato, CA: Norman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Colotla, V. A., & Bach-Y-Rita, P. (2002). Shepherd Ivory Franz: His contributions to neuropsychology and rehabilitation. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(2), 141–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Driver, J. (2001). A selective review of selective attention research from the past century. British Journal of Psychology, 92(1), 53–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finger, S. (1994). Origins of neuroscience: A history of explorations into brain function. New York: Oxford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strong. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6(8), 653–659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geschwind, N. (1979). Specializations of the human brain. Scientific American, 241(3), 180–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K., & Gelb, A. (1918). Psychologische Analysen hirnpathologischer Fälle auf Grund von Untersuchungen Hirnverletzter. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 41(1), 1–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldstein, K., & Scheerer, M. (1941). Abstract and concrete behavior an experimental study with special tests. Psychological Monographs, 53(2), 1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goltz, F. (1881). Discussion on the localization of function in the cortex cerebri. In W. MacCormac (Ed.), Transactions of the 7th international medical congress (Vol. I, pp. 218–228). London: J. W. Kolkmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodale, M. A., & Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15(1), 20–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harrower-Erickson, M. R. (1942). Kurt Koffka: 1886–1941. The American Journal of Psychology, 55(2), 278–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hebb, D. O., & Penfield, W. (1940). Human behavior after extensive bilateral removal from the frontal lobes. Archives of Neurology & Psychiatry, 44(2), 421–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., & Van den Abell, T. (1979). Right hemispheric dominance for mediating cerebral activation. Neuropsychologia, 17(3), 315–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman, K. M., & Van Den Abell, T. (1980). Right hemisphere dominance for attention: The mechanism underlying hemispheric asymmetries of inattention (neglect). Neurology, 30(3), 327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holmes, G. (1918). Disturbances of vision by cerebral lesions. The British Journal of Ophthalmology, 2(7), 353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. The Journal of Physiology, 160(1), 106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Inouye, T. (1909). Die Sehstörungen bei Schussverletzungen der kortikalen Sehsphäre: nach Beobachtungen an Verwundeten der letzten japanischen Kriege. W. Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jagaroo, V. (2009). Neuroinformatics for Neuropsychology. New York: Springer.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1950). In search of the engram. Society of Experimental Biology Symposium No. 4: Physiological Mechanisms in Animal Behavior. Cambridge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lauterbur, P. C. (1973). Image formation by induced local interactions: Examples employing nuclear magnetic resonance. Nature, 242(5394), 190–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lichtheim, L. (1885). On aphasia. Brain, 7, 433–484. [Reproduced in Lichtheim, L. 2006. Onaphasia. Brain, 129, 1347–1350.].

    Google Scholar 

  • Mazziotta, J. C., Toga, A. W., & Frackowiak, R. S. (Eds.). (2000). Brain mapping: The disorders. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mesulam, M. M., Van Hoesen, G. W., Pandya, D. N., & Geschwind, N. (1977). Limbic and sensory connections of the inferior parietal lobule (area PG) in the rhesus monkey: A study with a new method for horseradish peroxidase histochemistry. Brain Research, 136, 393–414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63(2), 81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parlee, M. B. (2012). Hans-Lukas Teuber: Envisioning neuropsychology. In A. Y. Stringer, E. L. Cooley, & A. L. Christensen (Eds.), Pathways to prominence in neuropsychology: Reflections of twentieth-century pioneers. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometric explorations of mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pruthenus, L. (1498). Trilogium animae.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raichle, M. (2008). A brief history of human brain mapping. Trends in Neuroscience, 32(2), 118–126.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (2009). The Halstead–Reitan neuropsychological test battery for adults—Theoretical, methodological, and validational bases. Neuropsychological Assessment of Neuropsychiatric and Neuromedical Disorders, 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Savoy, R. (2001). History and future directions of human brain mapping and functional neuroimaging. Acta Psychologica, 27, 9–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 20(1), 11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. (2001). Cognitive psychology: History. In N. Smelzer & P. Baltes (Eds.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral sciences (pp. 2140–2147). Oxford: Pergamon.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Squire, L. R. (2004). Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 82(3), 171–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages. Acta Psychologica, 30, 276–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toga, A., Clark, K., Thompson, P., Shattuck, D., & Van Horn, J. (2012). Mapping the human connectome. Neurosurgery, 71(1), 1–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toga, A. W., & Mazziotta, J. C. (Eds.). (2000). Brain mapping: The systems. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toga, A. W., & Mazziotta, J. C. (Eds.). (2002). Brain mapping: The methods. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. M., Watson, R. T., & Heilman, K. M. (1977). Discrimination and evocation of affectively intoned speech in patients with right parietal disease. Neurology, 27(10), 947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vesalius, A. (1543). De humani corporis fabrica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wernicke, C. (1874). De Aphasische Symptomen-complex. Breslau: Max Cohn & Weigert.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jordan P. Harp .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2017 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Harp, J.P., High, W.M. (2017). The Brain and Its Maps: An Illustrative History. In: Brunn, S., Dodge, M. (eds) Mapping Across Academia. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-024-1011-2_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics