Skip to main content

Interpreting Through Drawings

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Natural History Dioramas
  • 1070 Accesses

Abstract

Our present disposition to value wilderness is a consequence of its rapid disappearance. An awareness of nature conservation is also a relatively recent phenomenon in Malta. Malta is a small archipelago with limited endemic wildlife, and over a third of its surface area is occupied by building. The educational potential and role in biological learning of dioramas has been, quite recently, documented by various researchers (Ash, Curator 47(1):84–100, 2004; Piqueras et al. Journal of Museum Education 33(2):153–164, 2008; Reiss and Tunnicliffe, Paper presented at the Restructuring Science Education Through Research, 2007; Scheersoi, The important role of Natural History dioramas in biological learning 29:1–40, 2009; Tunnicliffe, Current Trends in Audience Research and Evaluation 18:23–31, 2005). Children stop to observe the exhibits, notice the different forms of animals and plants, the anatomical features of each organism and possible relationships between animals and plants or animals and animals. The child forms his or her concept of wildlife in general and more specifically a concept of the particular wildlife in the exhibit. At dioramas, children construct their own personal knowledge or mental model. Children’s attention is captured by particular features in natural history dioramas, where they pause and start to interpret what they see. Three grade 5 classes (63 pupils) composed of 9–10 year old primary students visited the Natural History Museum of Malta. After the diorama observation, the pupils were given boards to produce a drawing of their favourite diorama on A4 paper using HB and coloured pencils. Drawings were analysed using Altas.ti based on the NCT model of qualitative data analysis, where the three basic components are noticing things, collecting and thinking about things (Friese, Qualitative Data Analysis with Atlas.ti, London, Sage, 2012: 92). Results show which dioramas capture the children’s attention most. Also, which features in the dioramas are expressed in the drawings, particulary the animal types and physical features selected. Children explain their choice of diorama and influence on drawing as well as the limitations encountered while drawing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Anning, A. (1997). Drawing out ideas: Graphicacy and young children. International Journal of Technology and Design Education, 7(3), 219–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ash, D. (2004). How families use questions at dioramas: Ideas for exhibit design. Curator, 47(1), 84–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowker, B. (2007). Children’s perceptions and learning about tropical rainforests: An analysis of their drawings. Environmental Education Research, 13(1), 75–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coates, E., & Coates, A. (2006). Young children talking and drawing. International Journal of Early Years Education, 14(3), 221–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friese, S. (2012). Qualitative data analysis with atlas.ti. London: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gatt, S., & Vella, Y. (2003). Constructivist teaching in primary school. Valletta: Agenda.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golomb, C. (2004). The Child’s creation of a pictorial world (2nd edn.). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goodnow, J. (1977). Children’s drawing. London: Fontana Open Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Haney, W., Russel, M., & Bebell, D. (2004). Drawing on education: Using drawings to document schooling and support change. Harvard Educational Review, 74(3), 241–271.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harding, S. (2006). Animate earth: Science, intuition and gaia. Devon: Green Book.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hooper-Greenhill, E. (2000). Museums and the interpretation of visual culture. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hopperstad, M. H. (2010). Studying meaning in children’s drawings. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 10(4), 430–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Insley, J. (2007). Setting the scene. Museums Journal, 2(107), 33–35. Insley, J. (2008). Little landscapes: dioramas in museum displays. Endeavour, 32(1), 27–31.

    Google Scholar 

  • Louv, R. (2008). Last child in the woods. New York: Algonquin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mavers, D. (2003). Communicating meanings through image composition, spatial arragement and links in priamry school student mind maps. In C. Jewitt & G. Kress (Eds.), Multimodal literacy (pp. 19–33). York: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moran, E. F. (2006). People and nature: An introduction to human ecological relations. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peart, B., & Kool, R. (1988). Analysis of a natural history exhibit: Are dioramas the answer? The International Journal of Museum Management and Curatorship, 7, 117–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1967). The child’s conception of space (F. J. Langdon & J. L. Lunzer, Trans.). London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pipueras, J., Hamsa, M. K., & Edvall, S. (2008). The practical epistemologies in the museum: A study of students’ learning in encounters with dioramas. Journal of Museum Education, 33(2), 153–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, M. J., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (1999). Building a model of the environment: How do children see animals? Journal of Biological Education, 33(3), 142–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, M. J., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2007). Dioramas as depictions of reality and opportunities for learning biology. Paper presented at the Restructuring Science Education Through Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reiss, M. J., Tunnicliffe, S. D., Andersen, A. M., Bartoszeck, A., Carvalho, G. S., Chen, S. Y., et al. (2002). An international study of young peoples’ Drawings of What is inside themselves. Journal of Biological Education, 36(2), 58–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheersoi, A. (2009). Biological interest development at Natural History dioramas. The important role of Natural History dioramas in biological learning, 29, 1–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sjøberg, S. (2007). Constructivism and Learning. In E. Baker, B. McGaw & P. Peterson (Eds.), International encyclopaedia of education (3rd edn.). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. P., & Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2006). Bring back the nature table!. Environmental Education, 82, 8–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2002). The educational value of natural history collections in learing about biodiversity. The Biology Curator, 22, 27–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2005). What do dioramas tell visitors? A study of the history of wildlife diorama at the museum of Scotland. Current Trends in Audience Research and Evaluation, 18, 23–31.Tunnicliffe, S. D. (2007). The potential of natural history dioramas in developing science inquiry. Gemnews (Winter), 7.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Edward Mifsud .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Conclusions

Conclusions

The local habitat dioramas at the NHM in Malta are relatively small, placed in a narrow corridor with dim lighting and no panels. In comparison to Natural History dioramas in Europe and North America, those in Malta would seem quite unimpressive. Yet these dioramas attracted the attention of the visiting pupils who interacted with the exhibits actively. The boys and girls shifted from one diorama to the adjacent ones, while noting and commenting on what they were observing. Certain features in the dioramas grab the children’s attention who stop and interpret what they see. In this context, the dioramas may be considered a museum object described as an artefact containing animal and plant specimens in an ecological relationship. To Hooper-Greenhill (2000) objects do not exist outside interpretations of their meaning and significance. Their interpretation is rooted in existing experience and knowledge, while always being targets for feelings and actions (pg. 104). Person-object interaction leads to ‘situational interest’ that emerges in response to situational cues. In non-formal, free learning environments this situational interest is vital to learning. A more profound form of ‘individual interest’, that develops over time and exists within the person, may ensue from situational interest (Scheersoi 2009, p. 11).

Most of the pupils (90 %) managed to draw a diorama, while only two did not have a favourite, which indicates that pupils were positively influenced by the exhibits. The diorama as the object created the situational interest for the visit. A familiar place with children is internal yard of traditional Maltese houses and this could explain why the Rural Yard featured most frequently (33 %) in the drawings. Children seem to notice most features (22) in the rural yard diorama. However, the Agrifield and the Sand Dune were also commonly selected and represent two sites frequently encountered in the countryside and at the sea-side. This is another indication that interest arises from recognition of the familiar. It must be noted that choice of diorama was also affected by actual or perceived difficulty in drawing. It is crucial not to overlook the question of discrepancy between (cognitive) competence and (drawing) performance. Piaget and Inhelder have shown that there are performance problems associated with drawing (Piaget 1967, p. 71). We also need to consider children’s growing control over visual resources and their feeling of confidence in the situation (Hopperstad 2010, p. 432).

The predominance of animals is clear with almost half the number of items in drawings coded as animals. The general trend showing that animals are the most noticed and plants appreciably less was observed in each diorama type. A good number (75 %) of drawings featured a least one plant, but the total number of plants (19 %) was half that of animals (46 %). The apparent child disregard for plants has been previously reported in literature. Human artefacts (man made structures) seem to be more important to children than plants. The man made structures are quite prominent in the dioramas and so easily noticeable by the visitor. Also most of these items are familiar to children such as the colourful Maltese fishing boat or the rubble wall.

An interesting observation was that almost half (47 %) the drawings contained a feature not present in the dioramas. That is evidence of the tendency to insert organisms or objects from outside the diorama. For example, the beach diorama (showing the colourful boat) does not have a painted background of a blue sky with the sun and the palm trees, but the children drew them anyway. This study was conducted in Malta, an island where children are used to the predominant sunny weather with blue skies all year round and very limited cloud. It seems that the children produced a representation of a typical Maltese beach from their memory with the usual blue sky and the sun.

Apart from content, the drawings were also analyzed in relation to diorama composition. Composition refers to the degree of graphical closeness between the drawing and the actual diorama setting. Most drawings contained a low number of animals and plants present in the diorama. Few children included more than half the animals and plants in the diorama, while they tend to draw more of the physical features of the setting. Children missed the less conspicuous biota or omitted what they could not draw. There is a tendency to notice the larger animals or the unusual or unexpected.

Notably, most drawings (70 %) display all items in the same place as they occur in the diorama indicating an accurate spatial perception. The viewing of the dioramas acts as a trigger for children to assemble their related memories about the topic and compile a personal representation of the topic. In drawing, children record selective features that they find most relevant. These are generally connected with their personal experiences of everyday observations of animals around, media representations and narratives. Dioramas enhance situational interest when they induce emotional reactions and offer reference to allow different visitors to relate to prior experiences to the object observed.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2015 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Mifsud, E. (2015). Interpreting Through Drawings. In: Tunnicliffe, S., Scheersoi, A. (eds) Natural History Dioramas. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9496-1_20

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics