Skip to main content

Motivation and Creativity: Towards a Synthesis of Structural and Energistic Approaches to Cognition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology

Abstract

Cognitive scientists who model creative thinking on computers claim that the ability of their programs to replicate the discovery of scientific laws (e.g., Kepler’s third law from Brahe’s data) means that creative thinking in humans is nothing but problem solving of the kind computer heuristics use. This claim is shown to be a mystification based on a misunderstanding of creativity, on unrealistic replications of the initial conditions present at the inception of creative processes, and on a misleading identification of rationality with complex human thought processes. Some of the implications of such mystification for understanding thought processes in general are reviewed.

Reprinted from New Ideas in Psychology, vol. 6, no 2, pp. 159–176, 1988. Printed in Great Britain with permission from Elsevier © 1988 Elsevier.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 59.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 79.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    The research reported in this chapter was supported by the Spencer Foundation and by the Social Sciences Research Council Committee on Giftedness, Creativity, and the Learning Process.

References

  • Amabile, T. (1983). The social psychology of creativity. New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Binet, A. (1890). La concurrence des états psychologiques. Revue Philosophique de la France el de I’Etranger, 24, 138–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blackwell, R. J. (1983). Scientific discovery: The search for new categories. New Ideas in Psychology, 1, 111–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, B. (1985). Developing talent in young children. New York: Ballantine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brannigan, A. (1981). The social basis of scientific discoveries. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the holistic approach to behavior. In K. S. Pope & J. L. Singer (Eds.), The stream of consciousness. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1982). Toward a psychology of optimal experience. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 2). Beverly Hills: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1985). Emergent motivation and the evolution of the self. In D. Kleiber & M. H. Maehr (Eds.), Motivation in adulthood. Greenwich: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Getzels, J. W. Creativity and problem finding. In F. H. Farley & R. W. Neperud (Eds.), The foundations of aesthetics, art, and art education. New York: Praeger. (in press).

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Getzels, J. W., & Kahn, S. (1984). Talent and achievement: A longitudinal study of artists, 1963–1981. Unpublished Report to the Spencer and MacArthur Foundations, The University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1984). Being adolescent: Conflict and growth in the teenage years. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Larson, R. (1987). Validity and reliability of the experience-sampling method. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 526–536.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., Larson, R., & Prescott, S. (1977). The ecology of adolescent activities and experiences. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 6, 261–294.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Massimini, F. (1985). On the psychological selection of bio-cultural information. New Ideas in Psychology, 3(2), 115–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Robinson, R. (1986). Culture, time, and the development of talent. In R. Sternberg & J. L. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1919). How we think. Boston: Heath.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dewey, J. (1938). Logic: The structure of inquiry. New York: Putnam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckblad, G. (1981). Scheme theory: A conceptual framework for cognitive-motivational processes. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Einstein, A., & Infeld, L. (1938). The evolution of physics. New York: Simon & Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eysenck, M. W. (1982). Attention and arousal. Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. W. (1964). Creative thinking, problem-solving, and instruction. In E. R. Hilgard (Ed.), Theories of learning and instruction, 63rd yearbook of the national society for the study of education. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getzels, J. W., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1976). The creative vision: A longitudinal study of problem-finding in art. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Getzeis, J. W., & Jackson, P. W. (1962). Creativity and intelligence. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Groeger, J. A. (1987). Computation—The final metaphor? an interview with Philip Johnson-Laird. New Ideas in Psychology, 5, 295–304.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, H. E. (1986). The self-construction of the extraordinary. In R. Sternberg & J. L. Davidson (Eds.), Conceptions of giftedness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guilford, J. P. (1967). The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L., & Zacks, R. T. (1979). Automatic and effortful processes in memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 356–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henle, M. (1975). Fishing for ideas. American Psychologist, 38(8), 795–799.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hilgard, E. (1980). The trilogy of mind: Cognition, affection, and conation. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 16, 107–117.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, J. E., Nelson, B., Houck, M. R. (1983). The role of attentional resources in automatic detection. Cognitive Psychology, 51, 379–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Holt.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, C., & Larson, R. (1982). Bulimia: An analysis of moods and behaviors. Psychosomatic Medicine, 44, 341–351.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jung, C. G. (1960). On psychic energy (1928). In C. G. Jung (Ed.), Collected works (Vol. 8). Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kabira, K. (1986). Allocation of attentional resource: Giftedness and achievement. In F. Massimini & P. Inghilleri (Eds.), Selezione culturale umnana. Milan: Franco Angeli Editore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (1973). Attention and effort. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiesel, T. (1983). Scientific discovery: the larger problem situation. New Ideas in Psychology, 1, 99–109.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kubey, R. (1984). Television viewing experience in everyday life. Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1980). The significance of time alone in adolescent development. Journal of Adolescent Medicine, 2, 33–40.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., Csikszentmihalyi, M., & Graef, R. (1982). Time alone in daily experience: Loneliness or renewal? In L. A. Peplau & D. Perlman (Eds.), Loneliness: A sourcebook of research and theory. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larson, R., Zuzanek, J., & Mannell, R. (1986). Daily well-being of older adults with friends and family. Journal of Psychology and Aging, 1, 117–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipps, T. (1903). Leitfaden der Psychologie. Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, J. (1986). Experimental patterns and mathematical involvement in adolescence. In: F. Massimini & P. Inghilleri (Eds.), Selezione cullurale umana. Milan: Franco Angeli Editore.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nakamura, J. (1988). Optimal experience and the uses of talent. In: M. Csikszentmihalyi & I. S. Csikszentmihalyi (Eds.), Optimal experience: psychological studies of flow in consciousness. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neisser, U., Hirst, W., & Spelke, E. S. (1981). Limited capacity theories and the notion of automaticity: reply to Lucas and Bub. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 110, 499–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1976). Memory and attention. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Norman, D. A. (1980). Twelve issues for cognitive science. Cognitive Science, 4, 3–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1981). Intelligence and affectivity. Palo Alto, CA: Annual Reviews.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ribot, T. (1890). The psychology of attention. Chicago: Open Court.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, R. (1985). The daily experience of giftedness in adolescence: Sex differences and achievement. Paper presented at the American Educational Research Association meetings, Toronto, Ontario.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roe, A. (1946). The personality of artists. Educational and Psychological Measurement, 6, 401–408.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheffler, I. (1977). In praise of the cognitive emotions. Teachers College Record, 79, 171–186.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schlick, M. (1934). Uber das Fundament der Erkentniss. Erkentniss, 4. English edition: A. J. Ayer (Ed.), (1959). Logical positivism (trans: Ayer AJ). New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84, 1–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. R. (1980). The intentionality of intention and action. Cognitive Science, 4, 47–70.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory. Psychological Review, 64, 127–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1965). Decision making as an economic resource. In: L. H. Seltzer (Ed.), New horizons of economic progress. Detroit: Wayne State University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1978). Rationality as process and as product of thought. American Economic Review, 68, 1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon, H. A. (1985). Psychology of scientific discovery. Paper presented at the 93rd annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, Los Angeles, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Westfall, R. S. (1980). Newton’s marvellous years of discovery and their aftermath: Myth versus manuscript. Isis, 71, 109–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1902). Grundzuge der physiologischen Psychologie (vol 3). Leipzig.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1984). On the primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39, 117–123.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2014). Motivation and Creativity: Towards a Synthesis of Structural and Energistic Approaches to Cognition. In: Flow and the Foundations of Positive Psychology. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9088-8_11

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics