Skip to main content

Environmental Contributions to Language and Literacy Outcomes in Bilingual English-Arabic Children in the U.S.

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Handbook of Arabic Literacy

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 9))

  • 1569 Accesses

Abstract

Recent research supports the potent role of the environment in the development of language and literacy. While substantiated in research addressing language and literacy of many young children from various cultural and linguistic backgrounds, the role of the environment has received scant attention in research focusing on children who learn Arabic, a Semitic language characterized by diglossia. This chapter examines the role of environmental variables (parent education, beliefs, and home language use and literacy practices) in language and literacy outcomes of 64 bilingual English-Arabic children in the US. Results reveal a strong relationship between parent home language use and participantsʼ Arabic word reading, morphological awareness, and vocabulary skills; and between Arabic home literacy practices and Arabic reading comprehension skills. These findings corroborate previous cross-linguistic research and point to the importance of experience, namely parent language use and literacy practices, in predicting language and literacy outcomes in bilingual children. Implications for research, instruction, early intervention, and prevention of language and reading disorders in children acquiring Arabic are highlighted.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

References

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (2000). Effects of exposure to literary Arabic on reading comprehension in a diglossic situation. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 147–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2004). Reading and spelling error analysis of native Arabic dyslexic readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 651–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal reader Arabic. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 423–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D. (2006). Continuity in childrenʼs literacy achievements: A longitudinal perspective from kindergarten to school. First Language, 25, 259–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., & Levin, I. (2001). Mother-child joint writing in low SES: Sociocultural factors, maternal mediation, and emergent literacy. Cognitive Development, 16, 831–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aram, D., & Levin, I. (2004). The role of maternal mediation of writing to kindergartners in promoting literacy achievements in school: A longitudinal perspective. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 17, 47–61.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bialystock, E. (2007). Acquisition of literacy in bilingual children: A framework for research. Language Learning, 57(1), 45–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Green, D. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2009). Bilingual minds. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 10(3), 89–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bingham, G. E. (2007). Exploring the role of maternal literacy beliefs in mother child joint book reading and home literacy environments. Early Education and Development, 18, 23–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P., & Passeron, J. C. (1990). Reproduction in education, society and culture (2nd ed.). (trans: R. Nice). London: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyle, M., Georgiades, K., Racine, Y., & Mustard, C. (2007). Neighborhood and family influences on educational attainment: Results from the Ontario child health study follow-up. Child Development, 78(91), 168–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Branum-Martin, L., Mehta, P. D., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Cirino, P. T., Miller, J. F., & Iglesias, A. (2009). Pictures and words: Spanish and English vocabulary in classrooms. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(4), 897–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Ceci, S. J. (1994). Nature-nurture reconceptualized in developmental perspective: A biological model. Psychological Review, 101, 568–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (1998). The ecology of developmental processes. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human development (5th ed, pp. 993–1023). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. A. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 1: Theoretical models of human development (6th ed, pp. 793–828). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A. G., & van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Affective dimension of mother-infant picture book reading. Journal of School Psychology, 35, 47–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bus, A., van Ijzendoorn, M., & Pellegrini, A. (1995). Joint book makes for success in learning to read: A meta-analysis of intergenerational transmission of literacy. Review of Educational Research, 65, 1–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. In L. B. Feldman (Ed.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 189–209). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carreon, G. P., Drake, C., & Barton, A. C. (2005). The importance of presence: Immigrant parentsʼ school engagement experiences. American Educational Research Journal, 42, 465–498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chow, B. W., & McBride-Chang, C. (2003). Promoting language and literacy development through parent-child reading in Hong Kong pre-schoolers. Early Education and Development, 14(2), 233–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curenton, S. M., & Justice, L. M. (2008). Childrenʼs pre-literacy skills: Influence of mothers’ education and beliefs about shared-reading interactions. Early Education and Development, 19, 261–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L., & Scarborough, H. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contribution of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davison, M. D., Hammer, C., & Lawrence, F. R. (2011). Associations between pre-school language and first grade reading outcomes in bilingual children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 44, 444–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • DeBaryshe, B. (1995). Maternal belief systems: Linchpin in the home reading process. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 16, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeTemple, J. M. (2001). Parents and children reading books together. In D. K. Dickinson & P. O. Tabors (Eds.), Beginning literacy with language (pp. 31–53). Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • ECLS-K. (2011). Early Childhood Longitudinal program (ECLS). http://nces.ed.gov/ecls/kindergarten2011.asp.

  • Farran, L. K. (2010). The relationship between language and reading in bilingual English-Arabic children. Published Doctoral Dissertation, Georgia State University, Atlanta, Georgia.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farran, L. K., Bingham, G. E., & Matthews, M. W. (2011). The relationship between language and reading in bilingual English-Arabic children. Advance online publication Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal. doi:10.1007/s11145-011-9352–5.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farver, J. M., Xu, Y., Eppe, S., & Lonigan, C. (2006). Home environments and young Latino childrenʼs school readiness. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 21, 196–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Feitelson, D., Goldstein, Z., Iraqi, U., & Share, D. (1993). Effects of listening to story reading on aspects of literacy acquisition in a diglossic situation. Reading Research Quarterly, 28, 70–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 15, 325–340.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B., Anthony, J., Seals, L., & Mouzaki, A. (2002). Language development and emergent literacy in pre-school. Seminars in Pediatric Neuropsychology, 9, 173–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (2006). Becoming literate in Hebrew: The grain-size hypothesis and Semitic orthographic systems. Developmental Science, 9, 439–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genesee, F., & Geva, E. (2006). Cross-linguistic relationships in working memory, phonological processes, and oral language. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Report of the National Literacy Panel on K-12 Youth and Adolescents (pp. 169–177). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, C., Rueda, R., & August, D. (2006). Sociocultural influences on the literacy attainment of language-minority children and youth. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the National Literacy Panel on Language-Minority Children and Youth (pp. 269–347). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, N., Moll, L. C., & Amanti, C. (Eds.). (2005). Funds of knowledge: Theorizing practices in households and classrooms. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, J. E., Rivera, V., Davis, M., & Taylor, A. (2010). Foundations of young childrenʼs vocabulary development: The role of the home literacy environment (HLE). Early Childhood Services: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Effectiveness, 4, 69–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gwynne, O. K., Lengua, L. J., McMahon, R. J., & Conduct Problems Prevention Research Group. (2000). Parental involvement in school: Conceptualizing multiple dimensions and their relations with family and demographic risk factors. Journal of School Psychology, 38, 501–523.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, C. S., Detwiler, J., Blood, G. W., Qualls, C., & Detwiler, J. (2004). Speech-language pathologistsʼ training and confidence in serving Spanish-English bilingual children. Journal of Communication Disorders, 37, 91–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, C. S., Davison, M. D., Lawrence, F. R., & Miccio, A. W. (2009). The effect of maternal language on bilingual childrenʼs vocabulary and emergent literacy development during Head Start and kindergarten. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13(2), 99–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammer, C. S., Miccio, A. W., & Wagstaff, D. (2003). Home literacy experiences and their relationship to bilingual pre-schoolersʼ developing English abilities: An initial investigation. Language Speech and Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 20–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hao, L., & Bonstead-Bruns, M. (1998). Parent-child differences in educational aspirations and the academic achievement of immigrant and native students. Sociology of Education, 71(3), 75–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26(1), 55–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holes, C. (2004). Modern Arabic: Structures, functions, and varieties. Washington: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover-Dempsey, K. V., Battiato, A. C., Walker, J. M. T., Reed, R. P., DeJong, J. M., & Jones, K. P. (2001). Parental involvement in homework. Educational Psychologist, 36, 195–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Immordino-Yang, M. H., & Deacon, T. (2007). An evolutionary perspective on reading and reading disorders? In K. W. Fischer, J. H. Bernstein & M. H. Immordino-Yang (Eds.), Mind, brain and education in reading disorders. (pp. 16–29). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iraqi, J. (1990). Reading to Arabic-speaking kindergarten children compared to alternative enrichment activities as a means of improving listening comprehension and language skills. Unpublished Masterʼs Thesis, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Orthography, phonology, morphology and meaning. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, E., & Park. S. (1994). Home language as a predictor of academic achievement: A comparative study of Mexican- and Asian-American youth. The Journal of Research and Development in Education, 27, 88–94.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klesius, J. P., & Griffith, P. L. (1996). Interactive storybook reading for at-risk learners. The Reading Teacher, 49, 552–560.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S. H., Smith, K. E., Swank, P. R., Zucker, T., Crawford, A. D., & Solari, E. F. (2011). The effects of a responsive parenting intervention on parent-child interactions during shared book reading. Developmental Psychology. doi: 10.1037/a0026400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., & Aram, D. (2012). Mother-joint writing and storybook reading and their effects on kindergartenersʼ literacy: An intervention study. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 217–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Saiegh-Haddad, E., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2008). Early literacy in Arabic: An intervention study among Israeli Palestinian kindergartners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 413–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, J., Anderson, J., Anderson, A., & Shapiro, J. (2006). Parentsʼ beliefs about young childrenʼs literacy development and parentsʼ literacy behaviors. Reading Psychology, 27, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Machida, S., Taylor, A. R., & Kim, J. (2002). The role of maternal beliefs in predicting home learning activities in Head Start families. Family Relations, 51, 176–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G., & Parrila, R. (2011). Revisiting the home literacy model of reading development in an orthographically consistent language. Learning and Instruction, 21, 496–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manolitsis, G., Georgiou, G., Stephenson, K., & Parrila, R. (2009). Beginning to read across language varying in orthographic consistency: Comparing the effects of cognitive and non- cognitive predictors. Learning and Instruction, 19, 466–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (2004). Childrenʼs literacy development. London: Hodder Education.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J. R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Cheuk, C. S.-M., & Muse, A. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological and morphological awareness to reading in Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and America. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Chow, Y.-Y., & Tong, X. (2010). Early literacy at home: General environment factors and specific parentsʼ input. In D. Aram & O. Korat (Eds.), Literacy development and enhancement across orthographies and cultures (pp. 97–127). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • MacGinitie, W., MacGinitie, R., Maria, K., & Dryer, L. (2000). Gates-MacGinitie Reading Tests (4th ed.). Itasca: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

  • Midraj, S., & Midraj, J. (2011). Parental involvement and grade four studentsʼ Arabic reading achievement. European Journal of Educational Studies, 3, 245–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mohamed, W., Elbert, T., & Landerl, K. (2011). The development of reading and spelling abilities in the first 3 years of learning Arabic. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 24, 1043–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., de Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. J. H. (2008). Added value of dialogic mother-child book reading: A meta-analysis. Early Education and Development, 19, 7–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuman, S. (1996). Children engaging in storybook reading: The influence of access to print resources, opportunity, and parental interaction. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 11, 495–513.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Institute of Child Health and Development Early Child Care Research Network. (2005). Pathways to reading: The role of oral language in the transition to reading. Developmental Psychology, 41, 428–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Okagaki, L., & Bingham, G. E. (2006). Parentsʼ social cognitions and their parenting behaviors. In T. Luster & L. Okagaki (Eds.), Parenting: An ecological perspective (2nd ed., pp. 3–34). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paez, M. M., Tabors, P. O., & Lopez, L. M. (2007). Dual language and literacy development of Spanish-speaking pre-school children. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 28, 85–102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Kiefer, H. (2010). Morphological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: Within and cross-language effects on word reading. Reading and Writing: an Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 337–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ravid, D. (2006). Word-level morphology: A psycholinguistic perspective on linear formation in Hebrew nominals. Morphology, 16(6), 127–148. doi: 10.1007/s11525-006-0006–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reese, L., Garnier, H., Gallimore, R., & Goldenberg, C. (2000). Longitudinal analysis of the antecedents of emergent Spanish literacy and middle-school English reading achievement of Spanish-speaking students. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 633–662.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez, E. T., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2011). Trajectories of the home learning environment across the first 5 years: Associations with childrenʼs vocabulary and literacy skills at pre-kindergarten. Child Development, 82, 1058–1075.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rueda, R., & Garcia, E. (1996). Teachersʼ perspectives on literacy assessment and instruction with language-minority students: A comparative study. The Elementary School Journal, 96(3), 311–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 431–451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2012). Literacy reflexes of Arabic diglossia. In M. Leikin, M. Schwartz, & Y. Tobin (Eds.), Current issues in bilingualism: Cognitive and sociolinguistic perspectives. London: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2013). A tale of one letter: Morphological processing in early Arabic spelling. Writing Systems Research, 5, 169–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., & Geva, E. (2008). Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English-Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 481–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E., Levin, I., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2011). The linguistic affiliation constraint and phoneme recognition in diglossic Arabic. Journal of Child Language, 38, 297–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. B. Neuman & D. K. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook of early literacy research (pp. 97–110). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M. (2006). Testing the home literacy model: Parent involvement in kindergarten is differentially related to Grade 4 reading comprehension, fluency, spelling, and reading for pleasure. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 59–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & LeFevre, J. (2002). Parental involvement in the development of childrenʼs reading skill: A 5-year longitudinal study. Child Development, 73, 445–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., & Young, L. (2008). The effect of family literacy interventions on children’s acquisition of reading from kindergarten to grade 3: A meta-analytic review. Review of Educational Research, 78, 880–907.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sénéchal, M., LeFevre, J., Thomas, E., & Daley, K. (1998). Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 96–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. (2008). On the anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an outlier orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. E. (1992). The belief behavior connection: A resolvable dilemma? In I. E. Sigel, A. V. McGillicuddy-De Lisi & J. J. Goodnow (Eds.), Parental belief systems: The psychological consequences for children (2nd ed., pp. 433–456). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigel, I. E., & McGillicuddy-De Lisi, A. V. (2002). Parental beliefs are cognitions: The dynamic belief systems model. In M. H. Bornstein (Ed.), Handbook of parenting: Vol. 3. Status and social conditions of parenting (pp. 333–358). Mahway: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sonnenschein, S., Baker, L., Serpell, R., Scher, D., Turitt, V. G., & Munsertman, K. (1997). Parental beliefs about ways to help children learn to read: The impact of an entertainment or a skills perspective. Early Child Development and Care, 127/128, 111–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986). Mathew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading: Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38, 934–947.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strickland, D. S. (1990). Emergent literacy: How young children learn to read and write. Educational Leadership, 47(6), 18–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sulzby, E., & Teale, W. H. (1991). Emergent literacy. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. II, pp. 727–757). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2007). Using multivariate statistics (5th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Beacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tahan, S., Cline, T., & Messaoud-Galusi, S. (2011). The relationship between language dominance and pre-reading skills in young bilingual children in Eqypt. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 24, 1061–1087.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Steensel, R. (2006). Relations between socio-cultural factors, the home literacy environment and childrenʼs literacy development in the first years of primary education. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 367–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1962). Thought and language. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weigel, D. J., Martin, S. S., & Bennett, K. K. (2006). Mothersʼ literacy beliefs: Connections with the home literacy environment and pre-school childrenʼs literacy development. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 6, 191–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., & Byers-Heinlein, K. (2008). Bilingualism in infancy: First steps in perception and comprehension of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 144–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 30, 679–689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yeung, A. S., Marsh, H. W., & Suliman, R. (2000). Can two tongues live in harmony: Analysis of the National Education Longitudinal Study of 1988 (NELS88) longitudinal data on the maintenance of home language. American Educational Research Journal, 37, 1001–1026.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lama K. Farran .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Farran, L., Bingham, G., Matthews, M. (2014). Environmental Contributions to Language and Literacy Outcomes in Bilingual English-Arabic Children in the U.S.. In: Saiegh-Haddad, E., Joshi, R. (eds) Handbook of Arabic Literacy. Literacy Studies, vol 9. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-8545-7_16

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics