Skip to main content

Childhood, Play and the Evolution of Cultural Capacity in Neanderthals and Modern Humans

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology ((VERT))

Abstract

The life history pattern of modern humans is characterized by the insertion of childhood and adolescent stages into the typical primate pattern. It is widely recognized that this slowing of the maturational process provides humans with additional years to learn, transmit, practice and modify cultural behaviors . In both human and non-human primates a significant amount of their respective dependency periods are spent in play . In contrast to modern humans, Neanderthals experienced shorter childhoods. This is significant as there is extensive psychological and neurobiological evidence that it is during infancy, childhood and adolescence that milestones in social and cognitive learning are reached and that play and play deprivation have a direct impact on this development. Faster maturation rates and thus shorter childhoods relative to modern humans lessen the impact of learning through play on the connectivity of the brain . In the context of play behavior, humans are unique in that adult humans play more than adults of any other species and they alone engage in fantasy play. Fantasy play is part of a package of symbol-based cognitive abilities that includes self-awareness , language , and theory of mind. Its benefits include creativity, behavioral plasticity , imagination , apprenticeship and planning. Differences in the nature of symbolic material culture of Neanderthals and modern humans suggest that Neanderthals were not capable of engaging in human-grade fantasy play.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   39.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   54.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Baxter, J. E. (2005). The archaeology of childhood: Children, gender and material culture. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baxter, J. E. (2008). Archaeology of childhood. Annual Review of Anthropology, 37, 159–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, P. (2005). The role of play in the evolution of the great apes and humans. In A. D. Pellegrini & P. K. Smith (Eds.), The nature of play (pp. 13–26). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (1997). Evolutionary hypotheses for human childhood. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 40, 63–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (2003). The human pattern of growth and development in paleontological perspective. In J. L. Thompson, G. E. Krovitz, & A. J. Nelson (Eds.), Patterns of growth and development in the genus Homo (pp. 15–44). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogin, B. (2009). Childhood, adolescence, and longevity: A multilevel model of the evolution of reserve capacity in human life history. American Journal of Human Biology, 21, 567–577.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burghardt, G. (2005). The genesis of animal play: Testing the limits. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carruthers, P. (2002). Human creativity: Its cognitive basis, its evolution and its connections with childhood pretense. British Journal for the Philosophy of Science, 53, 225–249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casey, B. J., Giedd, J. N., & Thomas, K. M. (2000). Structural and functional brain development and its relation to cognitive development. Biological Psychology, 54, 241–257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (2012). Human Brains wire up slowly but surely. February 1, 2012. Science Now. http://news.sciencemag.org/2012/02/human-brains-wire-slowly-surely.

  • Clottes, J. (2010). Les mythes. In M. Otte (Ed.), Les aurignaciens (pp. 237–251). Paris: Éditions Errance.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conard, N. J. (2011). The demise of the Neanderthal cultural niche and the beginning of the Upper Paleolithic in southwestern Germany. In N. J. Conard & J. Richter (Eds.), Neanderthal lifeways, subsistence and technology (pp. 223–240). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2005). Working memory, its executive functions, and the emergence of modern thinking. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 15, 5–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coolidge, F. L., & Wynn, T. (2007). The working memory account of Neandertal cognition: How phonological storage capacity may be related to recursion and the pragmatics of modern speech. Journal of Human Evolution, 52, 707–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coqueugniot, H., & Hublin, J.-J. (2007). Endocranial volume and brain growth in immature Neandertals. Periodicum Biologorum, 109, 379–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coqueugniot, H., & Hublin, J.-J. (2012). Age-Related changes of digital endocranial volume during human ontogeny: Results from an osteological reference collection. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 147, 312–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Brain overgrowth in autism during a critical time in development: Implications for frontal pyramidal neuron and interneuron development and connectivity. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 153–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dean, C. M., & Smith, H. (2009). Growth and development of the Nariokotome youth, KNM-WT 15000. In F. E. Grine, J. G. Fleagle, & R. E. Leakey (Eds.), The first humans: Origin and early evolution of the genus Homo (pp. 101–120). New York: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durston, S., & Casey, B. J. (2006). What have we learned about cognitive development from neuroimaging? Neuropsychologia, 44, 2149–2157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finke, R., Ward, T., & Smith, S. (1992). Creative cognition. Boston, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, J.-C., & Martín-Andrade, B. (2005). Fantasy play in apes. In A. D. Pellegrini & P. K. Smith (Eds.), The nature of play: Great apes and humans (pp. 139–172). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunz, P., Neubauer, S., Maureille, B., & Hublin, J.-J. (2010). Brain development after birth differs between Neanderthals and modern humans. Current Biology, 20(21), R921–R922.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Green, R. E., Krause, J., Briggs, A. W., Maricic, T., Stenzel, U., Kircher, M., et al. (2010). A draft sequence of the Neandertal genome. Science, 328, 710–722.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harvey, P. H., & Clutton-Brock, T. H. (1985). Life history variation in primates. Evolution, 39, 559–581.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K., O’Connell, J. F., & Blurton-Jones, N. J. (2003). Human life histories: Primate trade-offs, grandmothering socioecology, and the fossil record. In P. M. Kappeler & M. E. Pereira (Eds.), Primate life histories and socioecology (pp. 204–231). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iriki, A., & Sakura, O. (2008). The neuroscience of primate intellectual evolution: Natural selection and passive and intentional niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, Biological Sciences, 363, 2229–2241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, H. S. (2002). Human life histories. In M. Pagel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of evolution (Vol. 2, pp. 627–631). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kahlenberg, S. M., & Wrangham, R. (2010). Sex differences in chimpanzees’ use of sticks as play objects resemble those of children. Current Biology, 20(24), R1067–R1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamp, K. A. (2001). Where have all the children gone? The archaeology of childhood. Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 8, 1–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konner, M. (2010). The evolution of childhood. Cambridge: The Belknap Press of Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuba, M. J., Byrne, R., Meisel, D. V., & Mather, J. A. (2006). When do octopuses play? Effects of repeated testing, object type, age, and food deprivation on object play in Octopus vulgaris. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 184–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, S. R. (2004). Brain growth, life history, and cognition in primate and human evolution. American Journal of Primatology, 62, 139–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leigh, S. R. (2012). Brain size growth and life history in human evolution. Evolutionary Biology, 39, 587–599.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, K. P. (2010). From landscapes to playscapes: The evolution of play in humans and other animals. In R. R. Sands & L. R. Sands (Eds.), The anthropology of sport and human movement: A biocultural perspective (pp. 61–89). Lanham, MD: Lexington Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Liu, X., Somel, M., Tang, L., Yan, Z., Jiang, X., Guo, S., et al. (2012). Extension of cortical synaptic development distinguishes humans from chimpanzees and macaques. Genome Research, 22, 611–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mace, R. (2000). Evolutionary ecology of human life history. Animal Behaviour, 59, 1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L., & Renfrew, C. (2010). The cognitive life of things. Recasting the boundaries of the mind. Cambridge: McDonald Institute for Archaeology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morin, E., & Laroulandie, V. (2012). Presumed symbolic use of diurnal raptors by Neanderthals. PLoS ONE, 7(3), e32856.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neubauer, S., & Hublin, J.-J. (2012). The Evolution of Human Brain Development. Evolutionary Biology, 39, 568–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowell, A. (2010). Working memory and the speed of life. Current Anthropology, 51, S121–S133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nowell, A. (2013). Cognition, behavioral modernity, and the archaeological record of the Middle and Early Upper Paleolithic. In G. Hatfield & H. Pittman (Eds.), The evolution of mind, brain, and culture (pp. 235–262). Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Museum of Archaeology and Anthropology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowell, A., & Chang, M. (2012). Symbolism in Late European Neanderthals: Detection and evolutionary context. Paper presented at the American Association for Physical Anthropologists meeting, Portland, OR.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paus, T. (2005). Mapping brain maturation and cognitive development during adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 60–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellegrini, A. D., Dupuis, D., & Smith, P. K. (2007). Play in evolution and development. Developmental Review, 27, 261–276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellis, S., & Pellis, V. (2009). The playful brain: Venturing to the limits of neuroscience. Oxford: Oneworld Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ponce de León, M. S., & Zollikofer, C. P. (2001). Neanderthal cranial ontogeny and its implications for late hominid diversity. Nature, 412, 534–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peresani, A., Fiore, I., Gala, M., Romandinni, M., & Taggliacozzo, A. (2011). Late Neandertals and the intentional removal of feathers as evidenced from bird bone taphonomy at Fumane Cave 44 ky B.P., Italy. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 108, 3888–3893.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. (1983). The codes of man and beasts. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 125–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ragir, S., & Savage-Rumbaugh, S. (2009). Playing with meaning: Normative function and structure in play. In R. Botha & C. Knight (Eds.), The Prehistory of language (pp. 122–141). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Robson, S. L., & Wood, B. (2008). Hominin life history: Reconstruction and evolution. Journal of Anatomy, 212, 94–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shea, J. J. (2006). Child’s play: Reflections on the invisibility of children in the Paleolithic record. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15, 212–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, P. K. (2010). Children and play. Malden, MA: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, T. M., Tafforeau, P., Reid, D. J., Pouech, J., Lazzari, V., Zermeno, J. P., et al. (2010). Dental evidence for ontogenetic differences between modern humans and Neanderthals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107, 20923–20928.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sofaer Derevenski, J. (1997). Engendering children, engendering archaeology. In J. Moore & E. Scott (Eds.), Invisible people and processes. Writing gender and childhood into European Archaeology (pp. 192–202). London: Leicester University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, J. L., & Nelson, A. J. (2011). Middle childhood and modern human origins. Human Nature, 22, 249–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999a). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999b). The human adaptation for culture. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 509–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vila, C., & Wayne, R. K. (1999). Hybridization between wolves and dogs. Conservation Biology, 13, 195–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vila, C., Savolainen, P., Maldonado, J. E., Amorim, I. R., Rice, J. E., Honeycutt, R. L., et al. (1997). Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic dog. Science, 276, 1687–1689.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vuontela, V., Steenari, M.-R., Carlson, S., Koivisto, J., Fjällberg, M., & Aronen, E. T. (2003). Audiospatial and visuospatial working memory in 6–13 year old school children. Learning and Memory, 10, 74–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wayne, R. K., Geffen, E., Girman, D. J., Koepfli, K. P., Lu, L. M., & Marshall, C. R. (1997). Molecular systematics of the Canidae. Systematic Biology, 46, 622–653.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, B. A. (1994). Dynamics of human reproduction: Biology, biometry and reproduction. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zilhão, J. (2007). The emergence of ornaments and art: An archaeological perspective on the origins of “Behavioral Modernity”. Journal of Archaeological Research, 15, 1–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zilhão, J., Angelucci, D. E., Badal-García, E., d’Errico, F., Daniel, F., Dayet, L., et al. (2010). Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the USA, 107, 1023–1028.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zimmermann, E., & Radespiel, U. (2007). Primate life histories. In W. Henke & I. Tattersall (Eds.), Handbook of paleoanthropology, vol 1: Principles, methods and approaches (pp. 1163–1205). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

I thank Miriam Haidle, Nicholas Conard, and Michael Bolus for inviting me to participate in the ROCEEH (The Role of Culture in Early Expansions of Humans) symposium at the University of Tübingen and for their support and patience. I also thank Helen Kurki, Felix Riede, and Tanya Smith for helpful suggestions. Finally, I am grateful to Iain Davidson for his encouragement and insightful comments throughout the process of writing this chapter.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to April Nowell .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2016 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Nowell, A. (2016). Childhood, Play and the Evolution of Cultural Capacity in Neanderthals and Modern Humans. In: Haidle, M., Conard, N., Bolus, M. (eds) The Nature of Culture. Vertebrate Paleobiology and Paleoanthropology. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-7426-0_9

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics