Skip to main content

Limits upon Orthographic Knowledge Due to Processes Indexed by Naming Speed

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: Neuropsychology and Cognition ((NPCO,volume 8))

Abstract

Research has clearly established the importance of phonemic sensitivity in reading acquisition, as reflected in measures such as the ability to segment words into constituent phonemes. Whether such skill plays as crucial a role in gaining orthographic knowledge, as it does in learning to decode words based on symbol-sound correspondences, is a matter of debate. Some theorists contend that poor development of letter-sound knowledge (Ehri, 1992) and insufficient reading experience or exposure to print (Stanovich, 1992) play the major roles in the orthographic skill deficits of reading disabled (RD) children. We wish to supplement this view by proposing that a largely independent individual difference factor, indexed by speed of naming simple visual symbols such as single digits or letters, has a significant effect upon learning and retrieving orthographic patterns. The reading disabled child typically has difficulties in both phonemic sensitivity and in orthographic processing. We argue that the reading disabled child’s failure to abstract orthographic regularity after repeated print exposure and consequent difficulty acquiring automatic word recognition (Venezky & Massaro, 1979) may be due to slow access to letter codes as well as to the spiralling effects of initially poor decoding and restricted reading experience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1981). What good is orthographic redundancy? In O.J.L. Tzeng and H. Singer (Eds.), Perception of print: Reading research in experimental psychology, (pp. 197–221 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to Read: Thinking and Learning about Print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allington, R.L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected reading goal. The Reading Teacher, 36, 556–561.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N.A. (1993). Phonemic awareness, naming, visual symbol processing, and reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 87–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, T.A., Torgesen, J.K., and Wagner, R.K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 334–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V.W. (1987). Global, component, and serial processing of printed words in beginning reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 43, 387–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V.W., Cartwright, A.C., Yates, C.M., Swanson, H.L., and Abbott, R.D. (in press). Developmental skills related to writing and reading acquisition in the intermediate grades: Shared and unique functional systems. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V.W., Yates, C., and Lester, K. (1991). Multiple orthographic codes in reading and writing acquisition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 3, 115–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. (1977–1978). Relationships between oral reading rates for letters, words, and simple text in the development of reading achievement. Reading Research Quarterly, 13, 223–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blachman, B.A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G. (1993). Text reading and rereading: Predictors of fluency beyond word recognition. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 133–153.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G. (1994). Tracing symbol naming speed’s unique contribution to reading disabilities over time. Paper submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G., and Kennedy, A. (1993). Effects of naming speed differences on fluency of reading after practice. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 318–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G., Steffy, RA., and Tate, E. (1988). Comparison of the effects of IQ control methods on memory and naming speed predictors of reading disability. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 304–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G., and Swanson, L.B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 195–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G., and Wolf, M. ( 1993, March). A double-deficit hypothesis for developmental reading disability. Paper presented to the meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P.G., and Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links between naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., and Bryant, P. (1985). Rhyme and Reason in Reading and Spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cornwall, A. (1992). The relationship of phonological awareness, rapid naming, and verbal memory to severe reading and spelling disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 532–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A.E., and Stanovich, K.E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 733–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A.E., Stanovich, K.E., and Wilson, M.R. (1990). Cognitive variation in adult college students differing in reading ability. In T.H. Carr and B A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., and Carpenter, P.A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 19, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dempster, F.N. (1981). Memory span: Sources of individual and developmental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M., and Rudel, R.G. (1974). Rapid “automatized” naming of pictured objects, colors, letters and numbers by normal children. Cortex, 10, 186–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M., and Rudel, R.G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C. (1992). Reconceptualizing the development of sight word reading and its relationship to recoding. In P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri, and R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 107–143 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L.C., and Wilce, L.S. (1983). Development of word identification speed in skilled and less skilled beginning readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 3–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N.C., and Miles, T.R. (1981). A lexical encoding deficiency I:Experimental evidence. In G.Th. Pavlidis, and T.R. Miles (Eds.), Dyslexia Research and its Applications, (pp. 177–215). John Wiley and Sons Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R., and Brown, I.S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 39–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R.H., Naylor, C.E., and Wood, F.B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics. Brain and Language, 39, 485–497.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R.H., Wood, F.B., Brown, I.S., and Campbell, S.K. (1987). Separate verbal memory and naming deficits in attention deficit disorder and reading disability. Brain and Language, 31, 171–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, J.R. (1981). Sources of process interactions in reading. In A.M Lesgold, and C.A. Perfetti (Eds.), Interactive processes in reading, (pp. 361–386 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golden, J. (1993). Analysis of orthographic processing skill and its relationship to rapid naming. Unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  • Golden, J., and Bowers, P.G. (1993, July). Knowledge of letter patterns and its relationship to reading skill. Poster presented to Meetings of the Canadian Society for Brain, Behavior and Cognitive Science, Toronto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1991). Learning about spelling sequences: The role of onsets and rimes in analogies in reading. Child Development, 62, 1110–1123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (in press). Towards an orthographic analogy model of reading development: Decoding vowel graphemes in beginning reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, M.D. (1980). Further evidence for a relationship between memory access and reading ability. Journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 19, 683–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, M.D., and McClelland, J.L. (1979). Processing determinants of reading speed. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 108, 151–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A.S. (1979). Intelligent testing with the WISC-R. NY: John Wiley and Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, A., and Bowers, P.G. ( 1991, August). The effect of practice on word identification speed in children. Poster presented to the Meetings of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, A. and Bowers, P.G. (1992, August). Individual differences in reading ability amongst university students. Poster presented to the Meetings of the Canadian Society for Brain, Behavior and Cognitive Science, Quebec City.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemoine, H.E., Levy, B.A., and Hutchinson, A. (1993). Increasing the naming speed of poor readers: Representations formed across repetitions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 297–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B.A., and Hinchley, J. (1990). Individual and

    Google Scholar 

  • developmental differences in the acquisition of reading skills. In T.H. Carr and B.A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 81–128). NY:Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M.W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill. Child Development, 58, 234–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M., Ransby, M., and Barron, R. (1988). Treatment, subtype, and word type effects in dyslexic children’s response to remediation. Brain and Language, 34, 328–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M.W., Warren-Chaplin, P.M., Ransby, M.J., and Borden, S.L. (1990). Training the word recognition skills of reading disabled children: Treatment and transfer effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 769–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F.R. (1985). Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 78–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F.R. (1993, March). A longitudinal study of print exposure, phonological skill and orthographic skill in normal and disabled readers. Paper presented at the Meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F.R., Szeszulski, P.A., Holt, L.K., and Graves. L. (1990). In T.H. Can, and B.A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 207–259 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. (1984). Review: Reading skill and language skill. Developmental Review, 4, 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, L.A., Pollatsek, A., and Well, A.D. (1988). Developmental dyslexia and word retrieval deficits. Brain and Language, 35, 11–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R.I., and Fawcett, A.J. (in press). Towards the origins of dyslexia. In R. Groner, R. Kaufmann-Hayoz, and S.F. Wright (Eds.), Reading and Reading Disorders: International Perspectives. North Holland: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Forsberg, H., and Wise, B. (1994). Genes, environment, and the development of orthographic skills. In V.W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge 1: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 27–71 ). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R.K., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B.J., and Foltz, G. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In G.E. MacKinnon, and T.G. Waller (Eds.), Reading Research: Advances in Theory and Practice: Vol. 4. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F., and Rack, J. (1990). Organization, heritability, and remediation of component word recognition and language skills in disabled readers. In T H. Carr, and B.A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 261–322 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, J., MacLeod, C.M., Hunt, E., and Davidson, J.E. (1985). Information processing correlates of reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 59–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E.J. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction. New York: CBS College Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1985). Reading Ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A. (1992). The representation problem in reading acquisition. In P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri, and R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading Acquisition (pp. 145–174 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C.A., Finger, E., and Hogaboam, T. (1978). Sources of vocalization latency differences between skilled and less skilled young readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 710, 730–739.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 36, 321–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1984). Sound priming in beginning readers. Child Development, 55, 406–423

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1989). Orthographic memory and learning to read. In P.G. Aaron, and R.M. Joshi (Eds.), Reading and Writing Disorders in Different Orthographic Systems. The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosner, J., and Simon, D.P. (1971). The auditory analysis test: An initial report. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 4, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S. (1992). Dyslexia in a computational model of word recognition in reading. In P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri, and R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 243–273 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M.S., and McClelland, J.L. (1989). A distributed, developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spring, C., and Capps, C. (1974). Encoding speed, rehearsal, and probed recall of dyslexic boys. Journal of Educational Psychology, 66, 780–786.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spring, C., and Davis, J.M. (1988). Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading. Applied Psycholinguistics, 9, 315–334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spring, C., and Perry, L. (1983). Naming speed and serial recall in poor and adequate readers. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 141–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1981). Relationships between word decoding speed, general name-retrieval ability, and reading progress in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 809–815.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1988). The dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590–604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P.B. Gough, L.C. Ehri, and R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307–342 ), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E., Feeman, D.J., and Cunningham, A.E. (1983). The development of the relation between letter-naming speed and reading ability. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 21, 199–202.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E., Nathan, R.G., and Zolman, J.E. (1988). The developmental lag hypothesis in reading: Longitudinal and matched reading-level comparisons. Child Development, 59, 71–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E., and West, R.F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 402–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, L.B. (1989). Analyzing naming speed-reading relationships in children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J.K., Morgan, S.T., and Davis, C. (1992). Effects of two types of phonological awareness training on word learning in kindergarten children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 364–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R., and Massaro, D. (1979). The role of orthographic regularity in word recognition. In L. Resnick, and P. Weaver (Eds.), Theory and practice of early reading: Vol. 1, (pp. 85–108 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., and Barker, T. (1994). The development of orthographic processing ability. In V.W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 243–276 ). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R.K., and Torgesen, J.K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin., 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, D.J., Price, G.G., and Gillingham, M.G. (1988). The critical but transitory importance of letter naming. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 108–122.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1974). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children: Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1984). Naming, reading, and the dyslexias: A longitudinal overview. Annals of Dyslexia, 34, 87–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 123–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Pfeil, C., Lotz, R., and Biddle, K. (1994). Towards a more universal understanding of the developmental dyslexias: The contribution of orthographic factors. In V.W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge I: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 137–171 ). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bally, H., and Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers. Child Development, 57, 988–1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P.H., Michel, G.F., and Ovrut, M. (1990). Rate variables and automatized naming in developmental dys12xia. Brain and Language, 39, 556–575.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. (1973). Woodcock Reading Mastery Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. (1987). Woodcock Reading Mastery Test-Revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yap, R., and van der Leij, A. ( 1991, August). Rate of elementary symbol processing in dyslexics. Poster presented at the Rodin Conference, Berne, Switzerland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yap, R., and van der Leij, A. (1993). Word processing in dyslexics: An automatic decoding deficit? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 261–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. (1993). Determinants of oral reading fluency and expressiveness in poor and average readers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, A. R., and Bowers, P.G. (1994). Individual differences and text difficulty determinants of reading fluency and expressiveness. Paper submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1994 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bowers, P., Golden, J., Kennedy, A., Young, A. (1994). Limits upon Orthographic Knowledge Due to Processes Indexed by Naming Speed. In: Berninger, V.W. (eds) The Varieties of Orthographic Knowledge. Neuropsychology and Cognition, vol 8. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-3492-9_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-3492-9_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-4460-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-017-3492-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics