Skip to main content

The Role of IQ in the Diagnosis of Reading Disorders: The Quest for a Subtype Based on Aptitude/Achievement Discrepancy

  • Chapter
Perspectives on the Classification of Specific Developmental Disorders

Part of the book series: Neuropsychology and Cognition ((NPCO,volume 13))

Abstract

In the history of the reading field, the quest to identify subtypes of reading disability has been as enticing as it has been futile. There is enormous face validity to the idea that poor readers differ among themselves in the way they have become poor readers and in the cognitive underpinnings of their disability. Yet the field has failed to progress beyond step one in defining separable groups of disabled readers — that is, subgroups who are behaviourally, genetically, and physiologically different from other poor readers.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 129.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 169.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aram, D., Morris, R., & Hall, N. (1992). The validity of discrepancy criteria for identifying children with developmental language disorders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 549–554.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Backman, J. E., Mamen, M., & Ferguson, H. (1984). Reading-level design: Conceptual and methodological issues in reading research. Psychological Bulletin, 96, 560–568.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Logie, R. H., & Ellis, N. C. (1988). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 30, 198–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (1979). Orthographic and word-specific mechanisms in children’s reading of words. Child Development, 50, 60–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. (1981). Reading skill and spelling strategies. In A. Lesgold & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Interactive processes in reading (pp. 299–327 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. (1986). Word recognition in early reading: A review of the direct and indirect access hypothesis. Cognition, 24, 93–119.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beech, J. R. & Awaida, M. (1992). Lexical and nonlexical routes: A comparison between normally achieving and poor readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 196–206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beech, J. & Harding, L. (1984). Phonemic processing and the poor reader from a developmental lag viewpoint. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 357–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dror, I., Pollatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identification in isolation and in context by college dyslexic students. Brain and Language, 40, 471–490.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bisanz, G. L., Das, J. P., & Mancini, G. (1984). Children’s memory for phonemically confusable and nonconfusable letters. Child Development, 55, 1845–1854.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. & Adams, C. (1990). A prospective study of the relationship between specific language impairment, phonological disorders and reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 1027–1050.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., Cain, M. T., & Ryan, S. M. (1992). A reading-level design study of phonological skills underlying fourth-grade children’s word reading difficulties. Child Development, 63, 999–1011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. E. (1978). Difficulties in auditory organization as a possible cause of reading backwardness. Nature, 271, 746–747.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E. & Goswami, U. (1986). Strengths and weaknesses of the reading level design: A comment on Backman, Mamen, and Ferguson. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 101–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cardon, L. R., Smith, S., Fulker, D., Kimberling, W., Pennington, B., & DeFries, J. (1994). Quantitative trait locus for reading disability on chromosome 6. Science, 266, 276–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carr, T. H. & Pollatsek, A. (1985). Recognizing printed words: A look at current models. In D. Besner, T. G. Waller, & G. E. MacKinnon (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 5, pp. 1–82 ). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47, 149–180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. (1989). Defining dyslexia as a developmental language disorder. Annals of Dyslexia, 39, 50–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. (1991). Early identification of reading disabilities. Topics in Language Disorders, 12 (1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W. (1993). The relationship between speech-language impairments and reading disabilities. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 948–958.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ceci, S. J. (1986). Handbook of cognitive, social, and neuropsychological aspects of learning disabilities (Vol. 1 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, K. N., Dale, P. S., & Mills, P. E. (1990). Defining language delay in young children by cognitive referencing: Are we saying more than we know? Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 291–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151–216 ). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). Individual differences in working memory and reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behaviour, 19, 450–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeFries, J. C., Fulker, D., & LaBuda, M. (1987). Evidence for a genetic aetiology in reading disability in twins. Nature, 329, 537–539.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Espy, K., Francis, D., Davidson, K., Rourke, B., & Shaywitz, S. (1989). Comparisons of cut-off and regression-based definitions of reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 334–338.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Rourke, B. P., Shaywitz, B. A., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1992). The validity of discrepancy-based definitions of reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 555–561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, J. M., Shaywitz, S. E., Shankweiler, D., Katz, L., Liberman, I., Stuebing, K., Francis, D. J., Fowler, A., & Shaywitz, B. A. (1994). Cognitive profiles of reading disability: Comparisons of discrepancy and low achievement definitions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 6–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. (1991). How early phonological development might set the stage for phoneme awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 97–117 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. & Snowling, M. (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 329–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. (1991). Anatomy of dyslexia: Argument against phrenology. In D. Duane & D. Gray (Eds.), The reading brain: The biological basis of dyslexia (pp. 119–131 ). Parkton, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. (1994). Developmental dyslexia and animal studies: At the interface between cognition and neurology. Cognition, 50, 133–149.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G., Rosen, G., Aboitz, F., & Geschwind, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1987). The processes underlying segmental analysis. European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology, 7, 462–464.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E. & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and language. Hove UK: Lawrence Erlbaum Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. & Bryant, P. (1989). The interpretation of studies using the reading level design. Journal of Reading Behaviour, 21, 413–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guthrie, J. T. (1973). Reading comprehension and syntactic response in good and poor readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 294–299.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Holligan, C. & Johnston, R. S. (1988). The use of phonological information by good and poor readers in memory and reading tasks. Memory & Cognition, 16, 522–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphreys, G. W. & Evett, L. J. (1985). Are there independent lexical and nonlexical routes in word processing? An evaluation of the dual-route theory of reading. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 8, 689–740.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hynd, G. S., Marshall, R., & Gonzalez, J. (1991). Learning disabilities and presumed central nervous system dysfunction. Learning Disability Quarterly, 14, 283–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hynd, G. S., Marshall, R., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (1991). Developmental dyslexia, neurolinguistic theory and deviations in brain morphology. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 3, 345–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N. E. & Butterfield, E. C. (1989). Reading-level-match designs: Myths and realities. Journal of Reading Behaviour, 21, 387–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jorm, A., Share, D., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (1986). Cognitive factors at school entry predictive of specific reading retardation and general reading backwardness: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 45–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kamhi, A. G. (1992). Response to historical perspective: A developmental language perspective. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 48–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kamhi, A. & Catts, H. (1989). Reading disabilities: A developmental language perspective. Austin: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen, P., Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., & Odegaard, H. (1990). MRI evaluation of the size and symmetry of the planum temporale in adolescents with developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 39, 289–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mamen, M., Ferguson, H., & Backman, J. E. (1986). No difference represents a significant finding. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 107–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Szeszulski, P. A., Holt, L. K., & Graves, K. (1990). Variation in component word recognition and spelling skills among dyslexic children and normal readers. In T. H. Carr & B. A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 207–259 ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Kliegl, R., Davidson, B., & Foltz, G. (1985). Individual and developmental differences in reading disability. In G. E. MacKinnon & T. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (Vol. 4, pp. 1–64 ). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Conners, F., & Rack, J. (1990). Organization, heritability, and remediation of component word recognition and language skills in disabled readers. In T. Carr & B. A. Levy (Eds.), Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 261–322 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Conners, F., Rack, J., & Fulker, D. (1989). Specific deficits in component reading and language skills: Genetic and environmental influences. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 339–348.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F. (1990). The genetics of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 31, 193–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Gilger, J., Olson, R. K., & DeFries, J. C. (1992). The external validity of age-versus IQ-discrepancy definitions of reading disability: Lessons from a twin study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 562–573.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Gilger, J., Pauls, D., Smith, S. A., Smith, S. D., & DeFries, J. (1991). Genetic and neurological influences on reading disability: An overview. Journal of the American Medical Association, 266 (1), 1527–1534.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., McCabe, L. L., Smith, S., Lefly, D., Bookman, M., Kimberling, W., & Lubs, H. (1986). Spelling errors in adults with a form of familial dyslexia. Child Development, 57, 1001–1013.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Van Orden, G., Kirson, D., & Haith, M. (1991). What is the causal relation between verbal STM problems and dyslexia? In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 173–186 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. (1985). Orthographic and phonetic coding in developmental dyslexia. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 325–340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 28–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynolds, C. R. (1985). Measuring the aptitude-achievement discrepancy in learning disability diagnosis. Remedial and Special Education, 6, 37–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodgers, B. (1983). The identification and prevalence of specific reading retardation. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 53, 369–373.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, H. & Dworkin, P. (1985). The language-impaired student. In P. Dworkin (Ed.), Learning and behaviour problems of school children (pp. 98–115 ). Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rubin, H. & Liberman, I. Y. (1983). Exploring the oral and written language errors made by language disabled children. Annals of Dyslexia, 33, 111–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M. & Yule, W. (1975). The concept of specific reading retardation. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 181–197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1743.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Semrud-Clikeman, M., Hynd, G. S., Novey, E., & Eliopulos, D. (1991). Dyslexia and brain morphology: Relationships between neuro-anatomical variation and neurolinguistic tasks. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 225–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Liberman, I., Mark, L., Fowler, C., & Fischer, F. (1979). The speech code and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 531–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. & Stanovich, K. E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education: Contributions from Educational Psychology, 1, 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Escobar, M. D., Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., & Makugh, R. (1992). Evidence that dyslexia may represent the lower tail of a normal distribution of reading ability. The New England Journal of Medicine, 326, 145–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, B. A., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., & Shaywitz, S. E. (1992). Discrepancy compared to low achievement definitions of reading disability: Results from the Connecticut longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 639–648.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, L. (1980). An evaluation of the regression discrepancy method for identifying children with learning disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 14, 79–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1988). Evidence that IQ scores are irrelevant to the definition and analysis of reading disability. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 42, 201–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1989). IQ is irrelevant to the definition of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 469–479.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1992). An evaluation of the discrepancy definition of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 618–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. (1993). The development of reading. In H. Reese (Eds.), Advances in child development and behaviour (pp. 63–97 ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S. & Ryan, E. B. (1988). Development of grammatical-sensitivity, phonological, and short-term memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 28–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, S. D., Kimberling, W. J., & Pennington, B. F. (1991). Screening for multiple genes influencing dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 3, 285–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. (1980). The development of grapheme-phoneme correspondence in normal and dyslexic readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29, 294–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Stackhouse, J., & Rack, J. (1986). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia: A developmental analysis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 3, 309–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986a). Cognitive processes and the reading problems of learning disabled children: Evaluating the assumption of specificity. In J. Torgesen & B. Wong (Eds.), Psychological and educational perspectives on learning disabilities (pp. 87–131 ). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1986b). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1988a). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 590–612.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1988b). The right and wrong places to look for the cognitive locus of reading disability. Annals of Dyslexia, 38, 154–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1991a). Discrepancy definitions of reading disability: Has intelligence led us astray? Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 7–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1991b). Word recognition: Changing perspectives. In R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 418–452 ). New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1992). Speculations on the causes and consequences of individual differences in early reading acquisition. In P. Gough, L. Ehri & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 307–342 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1993a). Does reading make you smarter? Literacy and the development of verbal intelligence. In H. Reese (Ed.), Advances in child development and behaviour (Vol. 24, pp. 133–180 ). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1993b). Dysrationalia: A new specific learning disability. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26, 501–515.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Cunningham, A. E. (1991). Reading as constrained reasoning. In S. Sternberg & P. Frensch (Eds.), Complex problem solving: Principles and mechanisms (pp. 3–60 ). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Nathan, R. G., & Zolman, J. E. (1988). The developmental lag hypothesis in reading: Longitudinal and matched reading-level comparisons. Child Development, 59, 71–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. & Siegel, L. S. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinmetz, H. & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Planum temporale asymmetry: In-vivo morphometry affords a new perspective for neuro-behavioural research. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 3, 331–343.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, J. (1988). Which aspects of reading ability show a “hump” in their distribution? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2, 77–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, J. (1992a). Genetics. In N. Singh & I. Beale (Eds.), Learning disabil- ities: Nature, theory and treatment (pp. 327–351 ). New York: Springer Verlag.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Stevenson, J. (1992b). Identifying sex differences in reading disability: Lessons from a twin study. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 4, 307–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szeszulski, P. & Manis, F. R. (1987). A comparison of word recognition processes in dyslexic and normal readers at two reading-age levels. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 44, 364–376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, H. G. & Schatschneider, C. (1992). Academic achievement following childhood brain disease: Implications for the concept of learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 25, 630–638.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (1985). Are there qualitative differences in reading behaviour between dyslexics and normal readers? Memory and Cognition, 13, 357–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill- Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. & Scanlon, D. M. (1989). Some prerequisites for interpreting results from reading level matched designs. Journal of Reading Behaviour, 21, 361–385.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, F. & Brown, G. (1992). Single-word reading in college dyslexics. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6, 263–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1998 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Stanovich, K.E., Siegel, L.S. (1998). The Role of IQ in the Diagnosis of Reading Disorders: The Quest for a Subtype Based on Aptitude/Achievement Discrepancy. In: Rispens, J., van Yperen, T.A., Yule, W. (eds) Perspectives on the Classification of Specific Developmental Disorders. Neuropsychology and Cognition, vol 13. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-2581-1_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-2581-1_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-4961-2

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-017-2581-1

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics