Skip to main content

Towards an Ecology of Language Acquisition

  • Chapter
Ecology of Language Acquisition

Part of the book series: Educational Linguistics ((EDUL,volume 1))

Abstract

Like any other scientific enterprise, the study of language acquisition (LA) evolves: the issues which dominate its agenda, the consensus on what constitute its `data’, the hypotheses that motivate its research programmes—all wax and wane in the cycles we know and expect in human affairs. At the end of the 20th century thinking in language acquisition research was showing signs, we believe, of a new kind of convergence. This volume aims to explore how a number of contemporary approaches and insights in LA research might be coherently interrelated through a perspective that can be called ecological. While much research on LA continues to consider the individual acquirer largely in closed-system terms, there is growing attention to the acquirer’s extensive interaction with their environment spatial, social, cultural and so on. Recent studies in such diverse fields as discourse analysis, linguistic anthropology, robotics, and cognitive semantics underline the heuristic value of the perspective promised in our title: ecology of language acquisition. In this introductory chapter we first offer an ecological critique of some dominant paradigms of LA research. We then go on to suggest how an ecological perspective motivates new approaches to acquisition issues, and how it informs each of the contributed chapters which follow. Our hope is that readers of all theoretical persuasions will find in this volume ideas, arguments and insights which, even if not woven into a fully-fledged theoretical fabric, at least point a way to fruitful theoretical reassessment.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Agar, M. (1985). Institutional discourse. Text, 5 (3), 147–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ard, J. & Homburg, T. (1983). Verification of language transfer. In S. Gass and L. Selinker (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp.157–167). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arundale, R. (1999). An alternative model and ideology of communication for an alternative to politeness theory’. Pragmatics 9 (1), 119–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bakhtin, M. (1981). The dialogic imagination. Austin, Texas: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bannink, A. (in press). Negotiating the paradoxes of spontaneous talk in advanced L2 classes. In C. Kramsch, (Ed.) (in press). Language socialization, language acquisition: Ecological perspectives. Continuum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barker, R. (1968). Ecological psychology. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barwise, J. & Perry, J. (1983). Situations and attitudes. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (1981). Roots of language. Ann Arbor, MI: Karoma Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloomfield, L. (1970). A set of postulates for the science of language. In C. Hockett (Ed.), A Leonard Bloomfield anthology (pp. 128–138). Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice (R. Nice, Trans.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1972)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1985). The genesis of the concepts of habitus and field. Sociocentrum, 2 (2), 11–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 513–531.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Broselow, E. & Finer, D. (1991). Parameter setting in second language phonology and syntax. Second Language Research, 7 (1), 35–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, D. (1993). After method: Toward a principled strategic approach to language teaching. in Matis, J. (Ed.), Strategic interaction and language acquisition: Theory, practice and research (pp. 509–520). Georgetown University Roundtable on Language and Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, P. (1999). Notes contributed to Round Table I (Language acquisition in cross-cultural perspective: challenges for research) at the Amsterdam Research Workshop on Ecology of language acquisition, University of Amsterdam, January 1999.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, E. (1943). Organismic achievement and environmental probability. Psychological Review, 50 (3), 255–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1985). Methodological preliminaries. In J. Katz (Ed.), The philosophy of linguistics (pp.80–125). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1986). Knowledge of language: Its nature, origin and use. New York: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1989). Some notes on economy of derivation and representation. MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, 10, 43–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1995). The minimalist program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Cambridge, MA: HarvardUniversity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., Engeström, Y., & Vasquez, O. (1997). Mind, culture, and activity: Seminal papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., Hood, L., & McDermott, R. (1997). Concepts of ecological validity: Their differing implications for comparative cognitive research. In M. Cole,Y. Engeström & O. Vasquez (Eds.), Mind, culture, and activity. Seminal papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (pp. 49–57). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cole, M., Hood L., & McDermott, R. (1978). Ecological niche picking: Ecological invalidity as an axiom of experimental cognitive psychology. Unpublished paper, The Rockefeller University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coffins, J. (1996). Socialization to text. In M. Silverstein & G. Urban (Eds.), Natural histories of discourse (pp. 203–229). University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, V. (1985). Chomsky’s Universal Grammar and second language learning. Applied Linguistics, 6, 1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, V. (1993). Linguistics and second language acquisition. Basingstoke: Macmillan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cook, G. (1997). Language play, language learning. English Language Teaching Journal, 51(3), 24–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, G. (2000). Language play, language learning. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crystal, D. (2001). Language and the Internet. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (1997), The symbolic species. New York: W.W. Norton Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ducrot, O. (1984a). La description sémantique en linguistique. In Le Dire et le dit (pp. 47–66). Paris: Ėditions de Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ducrot, O. (1984b). Structuralisme, énonciation et sémantique. In Le dire et le dit (pp. 67–94). Paris: Ėditions de Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dulay, H. & Burt, M. (1973). Should we teach children syntax? Language Learning, 23 (2), 245–258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dulay, H. and Burt, M. (1977). Remarks on creativity in language acquisition. In M. Burt, H. Dulay, & M. Finocchiaro (Eds.), Viewpoints on English as a second language. New York: Regents.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eckman, F. (1977). Markedness and the contrastive analysis hypothesis. Language Learning, 27, 315–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, J. (1994). Multilingualism. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (1998). Emergentism, connectionism and language learning. Language Learning, 48, 631–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Epstein, S., Flynn, S., & Martohardjono, G. (1996). Second language acquisition: Theoretical and experimental issues in contemporary research. Behavioral and Brain Sciences,19 (4), 677–758.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, F. & Schultz, J. (1981). When is a context? Some issues and methods in the analysis of social competence. In J. Green & C. Wallatt (Eds.), Ethnography and language in educational settings (pp. 147–160). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felix, S. (1985). More evidence on competing cognitive systems. Second Language Research, 1 (1), 47–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. (1977). Baby Talk as a simplified register. In C. Snow & C. Ferguson (Eds.), Talking to children: Language input and acquisition (pp. 219–235). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Firth, A. & Wagner, J. (1997). On discourse, communication, and (some) fundamental concepts in SLA research. The Modern Language Journal, 81 (3), 285–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rege, J., Bohn, O., & Jang, S. (1997). The production and perception of English vowels by native speakers of German, Korean, Mandarin and Spanish. Journal of Phonetics, 25, 437–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flynn, S. (1993). Interactions between L2 acquisition and linguistic theory. In F. Eckman (Ed.), Confluence: Linguistics, L2 acquisition and speech pathology. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J., (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frake, C. (1997). Plying frames can be dangerous. In M. Cole, Y. Engeström & O. Vasquez (Eds.) Mind, culture, and activity. Seminal papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (pp. 32–46). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gardner, R. (1985). Social psychology and second language learning. London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1979). Footing. Semiotica, 25, 1–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1974). Frame analysis. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of talk. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Green, J. & Wallat C. (1981). Ethnography and language in educational settings. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gumperz, J. (1981). Conversational inference and classroom learning. In J. Green & C. Wallatt (Eds.) Ethnography and language in educational settings (pp. 3–23). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanks, W. (1996). Language and communicative practices. Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (1993). Grounding symbols in the analog world with neural nets. Think, 2 (1), 12–78.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, R. (1990). On redefining linguistics. In H. Davis, & J. Talbot J. Taylor (Eds.) Redefining linguistics (pp. 18–52). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haugen, E. (1972). The ecology of language. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heath, S. (1983). Ways with words. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hernandez, A., & Bates, E. (1999). Bilingualism and the brain. In R. A. Wilson and F. C. Keil (Eds.), MIT encyclopedia of cognitive science (pp. 80–81). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hewstone, M. & Giles, H. (1986). Social groups and social stereotypes. In W. B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Intergroup communication (pp. 10–20). London: Edward Arnold.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutton, C. (1998). Linguistics and the Third Reich: Mother-tongue fascism, race and the science of language. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hymes, D. (1974). Foundations in sociolinguistics: An ethnographic approach. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irvine, J. (1996). Shadow conversations: The indeterminacy of participant roles. In M. Silverstein & G. Urban (Eds.), Natural histories of discourse (pp. 131–160). University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Itkonen, E. (1991). Universal history of linguistics: India, China, Arabia, Europe. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jordens, P. and Hulk, A. (2000). Retrieved August 22, 2000 from the web site of the Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kachru, B. (1996). The paradigms of marginality. World Englishes, 15, 241–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kellerman, E. (1983). Now you see it, now you don’t. In S. Gass and L. Selinker (Eds.), Language transfer in language learning (pp. 53–74). Rowley, MA: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramsch, C. (1993). Context and culture in language teaching. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kramsch, C. (Ed.), (in press). Language socialization, language acquisition: Ecological perspectives. Continuum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krashen, S. (1982). Principles and practice in second language acquisition. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lado, R. (1957). Linguistics across cultures. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lambert, W. (1974). Culture and language as factors in learning and education. In F. Aboud and R. Meade (Eds.), Cultural factors in learning and education (pp.79–106). Bellingham: Fifth Western Washington Symposium on Learning.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lamendella, J. (1977). General principles of neurofunctional organization and their manifestation in primary and nonprimary language acquisition. Language Learning, 27, 155–196.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lantolf, J. (2000). Sociocultural theory and second language learning. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen-Freeman, D. (1997a), Chaos/complexity science and second language acquisition. Applied Linguistics, 18 (2), 141–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larsen-Freeman, D. (1997b). Address at the closing lenary session of AILA 1996. AILA Review, 12, 87–92.

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen-Freeman, D. (in press). Discussion notes. In C. Kramsch (Ed.) (in press). Language socialization, language acquisition: Ecological perspectives. Continuum Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Larsen-Freeman, D. & Long, M. (1991). An introduction to second language acquisition research. Harlow UK: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. (1997). What’s special about experiments as contexts for thinking. In M. Cole, Y. Engeström & O. Vasquez (Eds.), Mind, culture, and activity. Seminal papers from the Laboratory of Comparative Human Cognition (pp. 57–70). Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lave, J. & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation. Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lenneberg, E. (1967). The biological foundations of language. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. (1983). Pragmatics. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, S. (1988). Putting linguistics on a proper footing: explorations in Goffman’s concepts of participation. In P. Drew & A. Wootton (Eds.), Erving Goffman: An interdisciplinary appreciation (pp. 161–227). Oxford: Polity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lightbown, P. & Spada, N. (1999). How languages are learned. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Long, M. and Sato, C. (1984). Methodological issues in interlanguage studies: an interactionist perspective. In A. Davies, C. Criper & A. Howatt (Eds.), Interlanguage (pp. 103–119). Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mackey, W. (1980). The ecology of language shift. In P. Nelde (Ed.), Languages in contact and in conflict (pp. 35–41). Zeitschrift fűr Dialektologie und Linguistiek 32. Wiesbaden: Franz Steiner Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Makkai, A. (1993). Ecolinguistics: Toward a new paradigm for the science of language? London: Pinter Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • McDermott, R. & Tylbor, H. (1983). On the necessity of collusion in conversation. Text, 3, 277–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, B. (1985). Second language acquisition in childhood. Vol.2: school-age children. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McLaughlin, B. (1990). The relationship between first and second languages: Language proficiency and language aptitude. In B. Harley, P. Allen, J. Cummins & M. Swain (Eds.), The development of second language proficiency. Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehan, H. (1979). Learning lessons. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehan, H. (1998). The study of social interaction in educational settings: Accomplishments and unresolved issues. Human Development, 41 (4), 245–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (N. Smith, Trans.). New York: Humanities Press. (Original work published 1945)

    Google Scholar 

  • Milroy, J. (1999). The consequences of standardisation in descriptive linguistics. In T. Bex & R. Watts (Eds.) Standard English: the widening debate (pp. 16–39). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • MITECS (2002). MII online Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science entry for ‘Cultural Psychology’. Retrieved May 30, 2002 from the World Wide Web: http://www.cognet.mit.edu/MIIECS/Front/topic_index.html.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan, C. (1925). Emergent evolution. London: Williams and Norgate.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mühlhäusler, P. (1996). Linguistic ecology: Language change and linguistic imperialism in the Pacific region. London: Routledge.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Nayar, P. (1994). Whose English is it? TESL-EJ, 1 (1), Fl.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelde, P. (1984). L’écolinguistique-aspects méthodologiques de la linguistique de contact. AILA Review, 1, 35–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.) (1979). Developmental pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. (1983). Acquiring conversational competence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • O’Grady, W. (1987). Principles of grammar and learning. University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pallotti, G. (1996). Towards an ecology of second language acquisition: SLA as a socialization process. In B. Wertens, T. Bongaerts & E. Kellerman (Eds.), Proceedings of EUROSLA 6. Toegepaste Taalwetenschap in Artikelen, 55, 121–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Patkowski, M. (1990). Age and accent in a second language: A reply to James Emil Flege. Applied Linguistics, 11, 73–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Philips, S. (1972). Participation structures and communicative competence: Warm Springs children in community and classroom. In C. Cazden, V. John & D. Hymes (Eds.), The function of language in the classroom (pp. 370–394). New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phillipson, R. & Skutnabb-Kangas, T. (1996). English only worldwide or language ecology? TESOL Quarterly, 30, 429–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, B. (1995). Social identity, investment, and language learning. TESOL Quarterly, 29, 9–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Poole, D. (1992). Language socialization in the second language classroom. Language Learning, 42 (4), 593–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Prüst, H. (1992). On Discourse Structuring, VP Anaphora and Gapping. PhD. Thesis, University of Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Quine, W. V. O. (1951). Two dogmas of empiricism. The Philosophical Review 60, 20–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rampton, B. (1995). Crossing: Language and ethnicity among adolescents. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rutherford, W. (1982). Markedness in second language acquisition. Language Learning, 32, 85–108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saussure, F. de (1966). Course in general linguistics (W. Baskin, Trans.). New York: McGraw-Hill. (Original work published 1916)

    Google Scholar 

  • Saville-Troike, M. (1982). The ethnography of communication. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schieffelin, B. (1979). Getting it together: An ethnographic approach to the study of the development of communicative competence. In E. Ochs & B. Schieffelin (Eds.), Developmental Pragmatics (pp. 73–108). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schumann, J. (1986). Research on the acculturation model of second language acquisition. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 7, 379–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schutz, A. (1967). Collected papers (Trans. M. Natanson). The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scollon, R. (1982). The rhythmic integration of ordinary talk. In D. Tannen (Ed.), Analyzing discourse: Text and talk (pp. 335–350). Washington D.C.:Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scollon, R. (1998). Mediated discourse as social interaction. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scollon, R. (in press). Cross-cultural learning and other catastrophes. In C. Kramsch (Ed.), (in press). Language socialization, language acquisition: Ecological perspectives. Continuum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scollon, R. & Scollon, S. (1981). Narrative, literacy and face in interethnic communication. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scollon, R. & Scollon, S. (1995). Intercultural communication. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scovel, T. (1969). Foreign accents, language acquisition, and cerebral dominance. Language Learning, 19, 245–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scovel, T. (1988). A time to speak: A psycholinguistic inquiry into the critical period for human speech. New York: Newbury House.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singleton, D. (1995). A critical look at the critical period hypothesis in second language acquisition research. In D. Singleton & Z. Lengyel (Eds.), The age factor in second language acquisition (pp. 1–29). Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D.I. (1967). A Field Manual for cross-cultural study of the acquisition of communicative competence. Berkeley, CA: Language-Behavior Research Laboratory.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sperber, D. & Wilson, D. (1988). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spolsky, B. (1989). Conditions for second language learning. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, H. (1983). Fundamental concepts of language teaching. Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, T. (1997). Theorizing language. Amsterdam: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Toolan, M. (1996). Total speech: an integrational linguistic approach to language. Durham, N.C.: DukeUniversity Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ungerer, F. and Schmid, H. J. (1996). An introduction to cognitive linguistics. Harlow, UK: Addison Wesley Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dam van Isselt, J. (1993). “Her name is—uh dat weet ik niet”: Authenticity in the L2 classroom. PhD Thesis. University of Amsterdam.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Dam, J. (in press). Ritual, face and play in a first English lesson: bootstrapping a classroom culture. In C. Kramsch (Ed.), (in press). Language socialization, language acquisition: Ecological perspectives. Continuum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Lier, Leo (1988). The classroom and the language learner. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Lier, Leo (1996). Interaction in the language curriculum: Awareness, autonomy and authenticity. London: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Lier, Leo (2000). From input to affordance: social-interactive learning from an ecological perspective. In J. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning (pp. 245–259). Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varenne, H. & McDermott, R. (1999). Successful failure: The school America builds. Boulder, Colorado: Westview Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, J. & Firth, J. (1997). Communication strategies at work. In G. Kasper & E. Kellerman (Eds.), Communication strategies: psycholinguistic and sociolinguistic perspectives (pp. 323–344). London: Longman

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinreich, U., Labov, W. and Herzog, M. (1968). Empirical foundations for a theory of language change. In W. Lehmann and Y. Malkiel (Eds.), Directions for Historical Linguistics (pp. 95–189). Austin: University of Texas Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wexler, K. (1991). On the arguments from the poverty of the stimulus. In A. Kasher (Ed.), The Chomskyan Turn. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, L. (1989). Universal grammar and second language acquisition. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Leather, J., Dam, J.V. (2002). Towards an Ecology of Language Acquisition. In: Leather, J., van Dam, J. (eds) Ecology of Language Acquisition. Educational Linguistics, vol 1. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0341-3_1

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-0341-3_1

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-6170-6

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-017-0341-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics