Skip to main content

Understanding How We Think about Space

An examination of the meaning of English spatial prepositions

  • Chapter

Abstract

People are quite successful at finding their way around in the world by following another person’s verbal directions. This is somewhat surprising given that a verbal description of a space is limited in the information it provides compared to the information provided by the direct perceptual experience of moving around in an environment. An important linguistic element by which we talk about spatial location in the English language is the closed-class set of terms called prepositions. There has been little experimental work on large numbers of spatial terms such as prepositions. In the present study the meaning of 25 English spatial prepositions was examined using multi-dimensional scaling techniques. The goal was to determine the most salient dimensions of spatial meaning, and where the most common English prepositions fall along these dimensions. English-speaking adults rated the similarity in meaning of pairs of prepositions across different contexts. One was a neutral (or no context) condition. Additionally, several contexts were generated by embedding the prepositions in carrier sentences containing different figure objects, ground objects and verbs. Four spatial dimensions emerged: verticality, containment, distance and one less easily interpreted dimension. The dimension of verticality was strongly evident in all contexts, and not much influenced by differences in context. The dimensions of containment and distance also emerged consistently across different sentence contexts, but order of individual words within these dimensions changed across different contexts. The results indicate that the most important dimensions that provide a framework for organising the meaning of English spatial prepositions are: verticality and containment. A dimension of distance also plays an important role. Context sometimes modifies the spatial meaning of certain prepositions. For example, the meaning of the word “through” implies containment in “The bird flies through the trees,” but not in “The bird is through the trees.” These results also demonstrate how multidimensional scales (MDS) can be used to study the meanings of a set of terms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Carlson-Radvansky, L. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1996). The influence of functional relations on spatial term selection. Psychological Science, 7(1), 56–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coventry, K. R., Carmichael, R., & Garrod, S. C. (1994). Spatial prepositions, object-specific function and task requirements. Journal of Semantics, 11, 289–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herskovits, A. (1986). Language and spatial cognition: An interdisciplinary study of the prepositions in English. In Studies in natural language processing, Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R., & Landau, B. (1991). Spatial language and spatial cognition. In D.J. Napoli & J.A. Kegl (Eds.), Bridges between psychology and linguistics: A Swarthmore festschrift for Lila Gleitman (p. 145–169).

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, L. V., & Wepman, J. M. (1966). A Spoken Word Count. Chicago: Language Research Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruskal, J. B., & Wish, M. (1978). Multidimensional scaling. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kucera, H., & Francis, W.N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). “What” and “where” in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 217–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., & Stecker, D. (1990). Objects and places: Syntactic and geometric representations of early lexical learning. Cognitive Development, 5, 287–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D., & Sadler, D. D. (1996). A computational analysis of the apprehension of spatial relations. In: P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, L. Nadel, and M. F. Garret (Eds.) Language and Space. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 494–529.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., & Johnson-Laird, P.N. (1976). Language and Perception. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shepard, R. N. (1962). The analysis of proximities: Multidimensional scaling with an unknown distance function. Psychometrika, 27, p. 125–240, 219–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Talmy, L. (1983). How language structures space. In Pick, H.L. & Acredolo, L.P. (Eds.) Spatial Orientation: Theory, Research, and Application. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Manning, C., Sera, M.D., Pick, H.L. (2002). Understanding How We Think about Space. In: Coventry, K.R., Olivier, P. (eds) Spatial Language. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-9928-3_8

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-9928-3_8

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-5910-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-015-9928-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics