Skip to main content

Grounding Meaning in Visual Knowledge

A Case Study: Dimensional Adjectives

  • Chapter
Spatial Language

Abstract

This paper originates from the idea that no complete account of lexical semantics can be given without a link between language and perception. It provides evidence for this claim by analysing two Italian adjectives (alto and basso — high/tall and low/short), which belong to a particular subclass of adjectives, namely those whose core meaning can be described by referring to some conceptualisation of our perceptual experience. Marr’s notion of 3D model and Jackendoff’s theory of frames of reference are used in order to represent referential properties of objects. The results of a test on the possible uses of alto (high/tall) and its antonym basso (low/short) are presented and discussed. These results, together with an analysis of the shapes of the objects tested, led to: (a) a hypothesis about the spatial conceptualisation of the objects involved; (b) a hypothesis about the way in which the procedures representing the meaning of alto and basso operate on the lexical entry of the modified noun (basically, which spatial feature they pick up); (c) a clear definition of context-dependence, as far as these adjectives are concerned, and a way to deal with it. The general hypothesis is that the meaning of these adjectives triggers a procedure which, on the basis of some characteristics of the shape of the object and the selected frame of reference, picks up a feature, the relevant vertical oriented axis (RVOA), that is present in the representation of the shape of the object, and changes the (fuzzy) values associated with it. Moreover, it enables us to derive adjectival selectional restrictions from some more basic referential property of the object involved. A computational framework to model the general hypothesis is presented and lexical entries and semantic procedures are described.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1992). Frames, Concepts, and Conceptual Fields. In A. Lehrer & E. F. Kittay (Eds.), Frames, Fields, and Contrasts. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1984). Bioprograms and the innateness hypothesis. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 72, 188–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Binford, T.O. (1971). Visual perception by computer. Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Systems and Control. Miami: Institute of Electrical and Electronic Engineering.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corrigan, R. (1989). Linguistic and non-linguistic categorization: structure and process. In R. Corrigan, F. Eckman & M. Noonan (Eds.), Linguistic Categorization. PhiladelphiaAmsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Devos, F. (1995), Still fuzzy after all these years. Quaderni di semantica 1, 47–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dirven, R., & Taylor, J.R. (1998). The conceptualisation of vertical space in English: the case of tall. In B. Rudzka-Ostyn (Ed.), Topics in Cognitive Linguistics. Amsterdam-Philadelphia: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goy, A. (1996). Semantica degli aggettivi: lo status quaestionis. Lingua e Stile, 2, 179–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harnad, S. (1993). Grounding symbolic capacity in robotic capacity. In L. Steels & R. Brooks (Eds.), The ‘artificial life’ route to ‘artificial intelligence’ — Building situated embodied agents. New Haven: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herskovits, A. (1986). Language and spatial cognition. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, D. D., & Richards, W. A. (1985). Parts of recognition. In S. Pinker (Ed.), Visual cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1987). Consciousness and the computational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1992). Languages of the mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1996). The architecture of the linguistic-spatial interface. In P. Bloom, M. A. Anderson, L. Nadel & M. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klein, E. (1980). A Semantics for Positive and Comparative Adjectives. Linguistics and Philosophy, 4, 1–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein, E. (1982). The Interpretation of Adjectival Comparatives. Journal of Linguistics, 18, 113–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G. (1987). Women, fire, and dangerous things. Chicago: Chicago University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., & Jackendoff, R. (1993). ‘What’ and ‘where’ in spatial language and spatial cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 217–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lascarides, A., & Copestake, A. (1995). Pragmatics of word meaning. In Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory (SALT5). Austin, Texas.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marconi, D. (1994). On the referential competence of some machines. In P. McKevitt (Ed.), Artificial Intelligence Review, 8, Special Volume on the Integration of Language and Vision Processing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marconi, D. (1997). Lexical competence. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision. San Francisco: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osherson, D. N., & Smith, E. E. (1981). On the adequacy of prototype theory as a theory of concepts. Cognition, 9. 35–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Partee, B. (1995). Lexical semantics and compositionality. In L. R. Gleitman & M. Liberman (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science: Language, vol. 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1985). Visual cognition: an introduction. In S. Pinker (Ed.), Visual cognition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pustejovsky, J. (1995). The generative lexicon. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E. (1975). Cognitive reference points. Cognitive Psychology 7, 532–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosch, E., Mervis, C., Gray, W., Johnson E., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 382–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, E.M., & Mervis, C. (1983). The effect of context on the structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 346–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsohatzidis, S.L. (Ed.) (1990). Meaning and prototypes. London-New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ullman, S. (1989). Aligning pictorial description: an approach to object recognition. Cognition, 32, 193–254.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wierzbicka, A. (1988). The Semantics of grammar. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wierzbicka, A. (1990). ‘Prototypes save’; on the uses and abuses of the notion of ‘prototype’ in linguistics and related fields. In S.L. Tsohatzidis (Ed.), Meaning and prototypes. London-New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zadeh, L.A. (1965). Fuzzy sets. Information and Control, 8, 338–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2002 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Goy, A. (2002). Grounding Meaning in Visual Knowledge. In: Coventry, K.R., Olivier, P. (eds) Spatial Language. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-9928-3_7

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-9928-3_7

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-5910-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-015-9928-3

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics