Skip to main content

Emergence of Language-Specific Constraints in Perception of Non-Native Speech: A Window on Early Phonological Development

  • Chapter

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 69))

Abstract

Adults have difficulty discriminating many non-native speech contrasts, yet young infants discriminate both native and non-native contrasts. Language-specific constraints appear by 10–12 months. Evidence presented here suggests that mature listeners’ discrimination is constrained by perceived similarities between non-native sounds and native categories, and that this native language influence may not be fully developed at 10–12 months. The findings suggest that young infants have broadly-tuned perception of phonetic details. Next, they begin to discern equivalence classes that roughly correspond to native phonemes. Perception of phonological contrasts, however, depends on recognition of their linguistic function, and thus develops later. But what sort of information in speech forms the basis for perception of equivalence classes or phonemic contrasts? I argue that distal articulatory gestures, rather than proximal auditory-acoustic cues or abstract phonetic features, are the primitives both for adults’ perceptual assimilations of non-native phones and for infants’ emerging recognition of native categories.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Aslin, R. N., & Pisoni, D. B. (1980). Some developmental processes in speech perception. In G. Yeni-Komshian, J. F. Kavanaugh, & C. A. Ferguson (eds.) Child phonology, Volume 2: Perception. New York: Academic Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T. (1978). The role of consonant and vowel acoustic features in infant cerebral asymmetries for speech perception. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Michigan State University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T. (1984). Discovering messages in the medium: Speech and the prelinguistic infant. In H. E. Fitzgerald, B. Lester, & M. Yogman (eds.) Advances in pediatric psychology. Volume 2. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T. (1988). TTie emergence of cerebral asymmetries in early human development: A literature review and a neuroembryological model. In D. L. Molfese & S. J. Segalowitz (eds.) Brain lateralization in children. New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T. (1990). Adult perception of nonnative contrasts differing in assimilation to native phonological categories. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, S177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T. (1991). Phonetic influences on the perception of nonnative speech contrasts by 6–8 and 10–12 month olds. Paper presented at the meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle WA, April.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T. (in press). The emergence of native-language phonological influences in infants: A perceptual assimilation model. To appear in H. C. Nusbaum & J. Goodman (eds.) The development of speech perception: The transition from speech sounds to spoken words. Cambridge MA MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T., McRoberts, G. W., & Sithole, N. M. (1988). The phonological basis of perceptual loss for non-native contrasts: Maintenance of discrimination among Zulu clicks by English-speaking adults and infants. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 345–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, C. T., & Strange, W. (1992). Effects of phonological and phonetic factors on cross-language perception of approximants. Journal of Phonetics, 20, 305–330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. A., Shankweiler, D., & Mann, V. A. (1983). Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 35, 345–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browman, C., & Goldstein, L. (1986). Towards an articulatory phonology. Phonology Yearbook, 3, 219–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Browman, G, & Goldstein, L. (1989). Articulatory gestures as phonological units. Phonology, 6, 201–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N., & Halle, M. (1968). The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper & Row.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cohn, A. (1990). Phonetic and phonological rules of nasalization. UCLA Working Papers, 76, May.

    Google Scholar 

  • Corina, D. P, Vaid, J., & Bellugi, U. (1992). The linguistic basis of left hemisphere specialization. Science, 255, 1258–1260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowder, R. G. (1971). The sound of vowels and consonants in immediate memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 10, 587–596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419–439.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flege, J. E. (1990). Perception and production The relevance of phonetic input to L2 phonological learning. In C. Ferguson & T. Huebner (eds.) Crosscurrents in second language acquisition and linguistic theories. Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fourakis, M., & Port, R. (1986). Stop epenthesis in English. Journal of Phonetics, 14, 197–221.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A. (1980). Coarticulation and theories of extrinsic timing. Journal of Phonetics, 8, 113–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A. (1986). An event approach to the study of speech perception from a direct-realist perspective. Journal of Phonetics, 14, 3–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A. (1989). Real objects of speech perception. Ecological Psychology, 1, 145–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A. (1991). Sound-producing sources as objects of perception: Rate normalization and nonspeech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, 1236–1249.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A., Best, C. T., & McRoberts, G. W. (1990). Young infants’ perception of liquid coarticulatory influences on following stop consonants. Perception & Psychophysics, 48, 559–570.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, C. A, & Dekle, D. J. (1991). Listening with eye and hand: Cross-modal contributions to speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 816–828.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fujisaki, H., & Kawashima, T. (1970). Some experiments on speech perception and a model for the perceptual mechanism. Annual Report of the Engineering Institute (University of Tokyo), 29, 207–214.

    Google Scholar 

  • Insabella, G., & Best, C. T. (1990). Four-year-olds’ perception of nonnative contrasts differing in phonological assimilation. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 88, S177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P. W., & Bertoncini, J. (1988). Viewing the development of speech perception as an innately guided learning process. Language & Speech, 31, 217–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keating, P. (1984). Phonetic and phonological representation of stop consonant voicing. Language, 60, 286–319.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinsbourne, M. (1978). The biological determinants of functional bisymmetry and asymmetry. In M. Kinsbourne (ed.) Asymmetrical functions of the brain. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A., Lacerda, F., Stevens, K. N., & Lindblom, B. (1992). Linguistic experience alters phonetic perception in infants by 6 months of age. Science, 255, 606–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D., & Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). Perception of the speech code. Psychological Review, 74, 431–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M., & Mattingly, I. G. (1985). The motor theory of speech perception revised. Cognition, 21, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., & Liberman, A. M. (1989). The alphabetic principle and learning to read. In D. Shankweiler & I. Y. Liberman (eds.) Phonology and reading disability: Solving the reading puzzle. Ann Arbor MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindau, M. (1982). Phonetic differences in glottalic consonants. UCLA Working Papers in Phonetics, 54, 66–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacKain, K. S. (1982). On assessing the role of experience on infants’ speech discrimination. Journal of Child Language, 9, 527–542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKain, K. S., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., & Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left hemisphere function. Science, 219, 1347–1349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macken, M. (1979). Developmental reorganization of phonology: A hierarchy of basic units of acquisition. Lingua, 49, 11–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCune, L., & Vihman, M. (1987). Vocal motor schemes. Papers and Reports in Child Language Development, 26.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264, 746–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Menn, L. (1983). Development of articulatory, phonetic, and phonological capabilities. In B. Butterworth (ed.) Language production, Volume 2: Development, writing and other language processes. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menn, L. (1986). Phonological units in beginning speech. In A. Bell & J. Hooper (eds.) Syllables and segments. Amsterdam: North Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P., Menn, L., & Silber, R. (1986). Early strategies for the perception and production of words and sounds. In P. Fletcher & M. Garman (eds.) Language acquisition. Cambridge UK: Cambridge Univ. Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. L., & Volaitis, L. E. (1989). Effect of speaking rate on the perceptual structure of a phonetic category. Perception & Psychophysics, 46, 505–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mohanan, K. P. (1986). The theory of lexical phonology. Boston: D. Reidel Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pisoni, D. B. (1973). Auditory and phonetic memory codes in the discrimination of consonants and vowels. Perception & Psychophysics, 13, 253–260.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, K. N. (1968). Acoustic correlates of place of articulation for stop and fricative consonants. Quarterly Progress Report (Research Laboratory of Electronics, M. I. T.), 92, 199–205.

    Google Scholar 

  • Studdert-Kennedy, M., & Shankweiler, D. (1970). Hemispheric specialization for speech perception. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48, 579–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Reenen, P. (1982). Phonetic feature definitions: Their integration into phonology and their relation to speech, a case study of the feature nasal. Dordrecht: Foris Publications.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vihman, M., & Velleman, S. (1989). Phonological reorganization: A case study. Language & Speech, 32, 149–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walton, G., & Bower, T. G. R. (in press). A modal representation of speech in infants. Infant Behavior and Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werker J. F. (1989). Becoming a native listener. American Scientist, 77, 54–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 49–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., Gilbert, J. H. V., Humphrey, K. & Tees, R. C. (1981). Developmental aspects of cross-language speech perception. Child Development, 52, 349–355.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Werker, J. F., & Lalonde, C. E. (1988). Cross-language speech perception: Initial capabilities and developmental change. Developmental Psychology, 24, 672–683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1993 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Best, C.T. (1993). Emergence of Language-Specific Constraints in Perception of Non-Native Speech: A Window on Early Phonological Development. In: de Boysson-Bardies, B., de Schonen, S., Jusczyk, P., McNeilage, P., Morton, J. (eds) Developmental Neurocognition: Speech and Face Processing in the First Year of Life. NATO ASI Series, vol 69. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8234-6_24

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8234-6_24

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-4251-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-015-8234-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics