Skip to main content

Why Faces are Special to Infants — on Connecting the Attraction of Faces and Infants’ Ability for Imitation and Cross-Modal Processing

  • Chapter
Developmental Neurocognition: Speech and Face Processing in the First Year of Life

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 69))

Abstract

There is more to faces than meets the eye. Infants can see the faces of others, but can also feel their own faces move. We propose a cross-modal hypothesis about why faces are attractive and meaningful to infants. According to this view, faces are attention-getting in part because they look like infants’ own felt experiences. This cross-modal correspondence drives not only visual attention but also action. Infants produce facial acts they see others perform. We here report an experiment on the efficacy of mothers versus strangers in eliciting facial imitation. The development of imitation is also investigated. The results show that there is no disappearance or “drop out” of imitation in early infancy; however, infants develop social expectations about face-to-face interaction that sometimes supersede imitation. Special procedures are required to motivate imitative responding in the 2- to 3-month age range. A theory is proposed about the motivation and functional significance of early facial imitation. According to this theory early imitation subserves a social identity function. Infants treat the facial behaviors of people as identifiers of who they are and use imitative reenactments as a means of verifying the identity of people. Facial imitation and the neural bases of the multimodal representation of faces provide interesting problems in developmental cognitive neuroscience.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Abravanel, E., & Sigafoos, A. D. (1984). Exploring the presence of imitation during early infancy. Child Development, 55, 381–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bower, T. G. R. (1974). Development in infancy. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bushnell, I. W. R., Sai, F., & Mullin, J. T. (1989). Neonatal recognition of the mother’s face. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 3–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L. B., & Strauss, M. S. (1979). Concept acquisition in the human infant. Child Development, 50, 419–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Schonen, S. (1989). Some reflections on brain specialisation in face processing. In A. W. Young & H. D. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of research on face processing (379–389). New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Schonen, S., & Mathivet, E. (1989). First come, first served: A scenario about the development of hemispheric specialization in face recognition during infancy. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 9, 3–44.

    Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (1990). Face agnosia and the neural substrates of memory. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 89–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Desimone, R., Albright, T. D., Gross, C. G., & Bruce, C. J. (1984). Stimulus-selective properties of inferior temporal neurons in the macaque. Journal of Neuroscience, 4, 2051–2068.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, H. D., & Young, A. W. (1989). Are faces special? In A. W. Young & H. D. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of research on face processing (1–26). New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M., Cohen, D., Garcia, R., & Greenberg, R. (1984). Mother-stranger face discrimination by the newborn. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 19–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Field, T. M., Woodson, R., Greenberg, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flin, R., & Dziurawiec, S. (1989). Developmental factors in face processing. In A. W. Young & H. D. Ellis (Eds.), Handbook of research on face processing (pp. 335–378). New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fontaine, R. (1984). Imitative skills between birth and six months. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 323–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, E. J., & Walker, A. S. (1984). Development of knowledge of visual-tactual affordances of substance. Child Development, 55, 453–460.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gross, C. G., & Rodman, H. (this volume). When do face detectors develop in monkeys? In de Boysson-Bardies, B., de Schonen, S., Jusczyk, P., MacNeilage, P., & Morton, J. Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face processing in the first year of life. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Acdemic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heimann, M., Nelson, K. E., & Schaller, J. (1989). Neonatal imitation of tongue protrusion and mouth opening: Methodological aspects and evidence of early individual differences. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 30, 90–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., & Morton, J. (1991). Biology and cognitive development: The case of face recognition. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaitz, M., Meschulach-Sarfaty, O., Auerbach, J., & Eidelman, A. (1988). A reexamination of newborn’s ability to imitate facial expressions. Developmental Psychology, 24, 3–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiner, K. A. (1987). Amplitude and phase spectra as indices of infants’ pattern preferences. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kleiner, K. A., & Banks, M. S. (1987). Stimulus energy does not account for 2-month-olds’ face preferences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 594–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K. (1983). Perception of auditory equivalence classes for speech in early infancy. Infant Behavior and Development, 6, 263–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. Science, 218, 1138–1141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1984). The intermodal representation of speech in infants. Infant Behavior and Development, 7, 361–381.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P.K., & Meltzoff, A.N. (1988). Speech as an intermodal object of perception. In A. Yonas (Ed.), Perceptual development in infancy: The Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology (Vol. 20, pp. 235–266). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, P. K., Williams, K. A, & Meltzoff, A. N. (1991). Cross-modal speech perception in adults and infants using nonspeech auditory stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 829–840.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Legerstee, M. (1991). The role of person and object in eliciting early imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 423–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKain, K., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Spieker, S., & Stern, D. (1983). Infant intermodal speech perception is a left-hemisphere function. Science, 219, 1347–1349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maratos, O. (1982). Trends in the development of imitation in early infancy. In T. G. Bever (Ed.), Regressions in mental development: Basic phenomena and theories (pp. 81–101). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maurer, D. (1985). Infants’ perception of facedness. In T. M. Field & N. A. Fox (Eds.), Social perception in infants (pp. 73–100). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Borton, R. W. (1979). Intermodal matching by human neonates. Nature, 282, 403–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., Kuhl, P. K., & Moore, M. K. (1991). Perception, representation, and the control of action in newborns and young infants: Toward a new synthesis. In M. J. S. Weiss & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), Newborn attention: Biological constraints and the influence of experience (pp. 377–411). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1985). Cognitive foundations and social functions of imitation and intermodal representation in infancy. In J. Mehler & R. Fox (Eds.), Neonate cognition: Beyond the blooming, buzzing confusion (pp. 139–156). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N, & Moore, M. K. (1989). Imitation in newborn infants: Exploring the range of gestures imitated and the underlying mechanisms. Developmental Psychology, 25, 954–962.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1992). Early imitation within a functional framework: The importance of person identity, movement, and development. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 479–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (In press). Imitation, memory, and representation of persons in 6-week-old infants. Infant Development and Behavior.

    Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A. (1962). Causalité, permanence, et réalité phénoménales. Louvain: Publications Universitaires.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moore, M. K., Borton, R., & Darby, B. L. (1978). Visual tracking in young infants: Evidence for object identity or object permanence? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 25, 183–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, M. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1978). Object permanence, imitation, and language development in infancy: Toward a neo-Piagetian perspective on communicative and cognitive development. In F. D. Minifie & L. L. Lloyd (Eds.), Communicative and cognitive abilities — Early behavioral assessment (pp. 151–184). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). CONSPEC and CONLERN: A two-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pascalis, O., Deruelle, C, Fabre-Grenet, M., de Schonen, S., Morton, J., & Johnson, M. (1992). Mother-stranger discrimination: Changing the outer face contour suppresses preference for mother’s face in 4-day-olds. Presented at the Second Meeting of the Cognitive Studies Program, Marseille, France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perret, D. I., Mistlin, A. J., & Chitty, A. J. (1987). Visual neurones responsive to faces. Trends in Neurosciences, 1987, 10, 358–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1954). The construction of reality in the child. New York: Basic Books.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reissland, N. (1988). Neonatal imitation in the first hour of life: Observations in rural Nepal. Developmental Psychology, 24, 464–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stein, B. E., & Meredith, M. A. (In press). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Strawson, P. F. (1959). Individuals: An essay in descriptive metaphysics. London: Methuen.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A. (1986). The role of movement in eliciting early imitations. Child Development, 57, 66–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, G. E., Bower, N. J. A., & Bower, T. G. R. (1992). Recognition of familiar faces by newborns. Infant Behavior and Development, 15, 265–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Walton, G. E., & Bower, T. G. R. (In press). Amodal representation of speech by infants. Infant Behavior and Development.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1993 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Meltzoff, A.N., Moore, M.K. (1993). Why Faces are Special to Infants — on Connecting the Attraction of Faces and Infants’ Ability for Imitation and Cross-Modal Processing. In: de Boysson-Bardies, B., de Schonen, S., Jusczyk, P., McNeilage, P., Morton, J. (eds) Developmental Neurocognition: Speech and Face Processing in the First Year of Life. NATO ASI Series, vol 69. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8234-6_18

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-8234-6_18

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-90-481-4251-4

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-015-8234-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics