Skip to main content

The Role of Semantic and Phonological Skills in Learning to Read: Implications for Assessment and Teaching

  • Chapter
Learning to Read: An Integrated View from Research and Practice

Part of the book series: Neuropsychology and Cognition ((NPCO,volume 17))

Abstract

Most theories of reading development stress the importance of phonological awareness to the acquisition of reading skill. These theories are supported by findings that pre-school measures of phonological awareness predict later reading success and that training phonological skills benefits reading development (Goswami and Bryant, 1990; Brady and Shankweiler, 1991 for reviews). The corollary of this is that poor readers show phonological processing deficits which are a plausible cause of their reading problems (Shankweiler et al, 1995; Stanovich and Siegel, 1994; Snowling, 1995). However, becoming a skilled reader requires more than just phonological awareness. As Gough and Tunmer (1986; see also Tunmer and Hoover, 1992) have argued, reading is well described as the product of two relatively independent skills, decoding and linguistic comprehension. Both of these skills are necessary, but neither alone is sufficient, for successful reading. It follows from Gough and Tunmer’s argument that children can have difficulties with either of these fundamental aspects of reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Adams, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brady, S. and Shankweiler, D. (1991) Phonological processes in literacy. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B. and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1995). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 2-and 3-year follow-up and a new preschool trial. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 488–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, L.J., Chapman, J.P., Curran, T.E. and Miller, M.B. (1994). Do children and the elderly show heightened semantic priming? How to answer the question. Developmental Review, 14, 159–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. and Bryant, P.E. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. Hove: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P.B. and Tunmer, W.E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, P., Hulme, C. and Ellis, A.W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: The phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 41–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C. (1995). The messenger may be wrong, but the message may be right. Journal for Research in Reading, 18, 146–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirk, S.A., McCarthy, J.J. and Kirk, W. (1968). The Illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities — revised. University of Illinois Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindamood, C. and Lindamood, P. (1975). Auditory discrimination in depth. Columbus, OH: MacMillan/McGraw Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M.W., Borden, S.L., DeLuca, T., Lacreerenza, L., Benson, N.J. and Brackstone, D. (1994). Treating the core deficits of developmental dyslexia: Evidence of transfer of learning after phonologically-and strategy-based reading programs. Developmental Psychology, 30, 805–822.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M.W., Warren-Chaplin, P.M., Ransby, M.J. and Borden, S.L. (1990). Training the word recognition skills of dyslexic children: Treatment and transfer effects. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 796–780.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDougall, S., Hulme, C., Ellis, A.W. and Monk, A. (1994). Learning to read: The role of short-term memory and phonological skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 112–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V. and Snowling, M.J. (1998). Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading: The role of metalinguistic and short-term memory skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 33, 320–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M. and Taylor, S. (1997). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 65, 370–396.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K. and Snowling, M. (in press). Assessing reading difficulties: The validity and utility of current measures of reading skill. British Journal of Educational Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K. and Snowling, M. (1998). Individual differences in contextual facilitation: Evidence from dyslexia and poor reading comprehension. Child Development, 69, 996–1011.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Neale, M.D. (1989). The Neale analysis of reading ability — revised. Windsor, UK: NFER.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Wise, B., Johnson, M. and Ring, J. (in press). The etiology and remediation of phonologically based word recognition and spelling disabilities: Are phonological deficits the “hole” story?. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Palinscar, A.S. and Brown, A.L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117–175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D., McClelland, J.L., Seidenberg, M.S. and Patterson, K.E. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pring, L. and Snowling, M. (1986). Developmental changes in word recognition: An information-processing account. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38A, 395–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rego, L.L. and Bryant, P.E. (1993). The connection between phonological, syntactic and semantic skills and children’s reading and spelling. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 13, 235–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rust, J., Golombok, S. and Trickey, G. (1992). Wechsler objective reading dimensions. The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semel, E., Wiig, E.H. and Secord, W. (1987). Clinical evaluation of language fundamentals — revised. The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L., Fowler, A.E., Liberman, A.M., Brady, S.A., Thornton, R., Lundquist, E., Dreyer, L., Fletcher, J.M., Stubing, K.K., Shaywitz, S.E. and Shaywitz, B.A. (1995). Cognitive profiles of reading disabled children: Comparison of language skills in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Psychological Science, 6, 149–156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D.L. and Stanovich, K.E. (1995). Cognitive processes in early reading development: Accommodating individual differences into a model of acquisition. Issues in Education, 1, 1–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. (1995). Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 132–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. (1996). Annotation: Contemporary approaches to the teaching of reading. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 139–148.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Hulme, C., Smith, A. and Thomas, J. (1994). The effects of phonetic similarity and word length on children’s sound categorisation performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 160–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Stothard, S.E. and McLean, J. (1996). The graded nonword reading test. Thames Valley Test Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 16, 32–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K.E. and Siegel, L.S. (1994). The phenotypic performance profile of reading-disabled children: A regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 24–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S.E. (1992). Reading difficulties in children: Problems of decoding and comprehension. Unpublished DPhil Thesis, University of York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S.E. and Hulme, C. (1992). Reading comprehension difficulties in children: The role of language comprehension and working memory skills. Reading and Writing, 4, 245–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stothard, S.E. and Huhne, C. (1995). A comparison of phonological skills inchildren with reading comprehension difficulties and children with decoding difficulties. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 36, 399–408.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Mullennix J., BijeLjac-Babic, R. and Richmond-Welty, D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W.E. and Chapman, J.W. (1998). Language prediction skill, phonological recoding ability and beginning reading. In M. Joshi and C. Hulme (Eds.), Cognitive basis of reading, writing and spelling (pp. 33–68). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W.E. and Hoover, W.A. (1992). Cognitive and linguistic factors in learning to read. In P. Gough, L. Ehri and R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 175–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1999 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Snowling, M., Nation, K., Muter, V. (1999). The Role of Semantic and Phonological Skills in Learning to Read: Implications for Assessment and Teaching. In: Nunes, T. (eds) Learning to Read: An Integrated View from Research and Practice. Neuropsychology and Cognition, vol 17. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-4826-9_11

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-4826-9_11

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-0-7923-5992-0

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-011-4826-9

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics