Advertisement

Avoid the Pedestrian’s Paradox

  • Edward P. StablerJr.
Chapter
Part of the Studies in Linguistics and Philosophy book series (SLAP, volume 44)

Abstract

Mark Steedman (1989) has suggested that each of the following assumptions about human language understanding is appealing, but that they lead to a paradox when taken together:
  1. 1.

    Incremental comprehension: Human natural language understanding is serial and incremental: the words of a sentence are interpreted rapidly as they are heard or read.

     
  2. 2.

    Right-branching syntactic structures: English, like other SVO and SOV languages, has predominantly right-branching syntactic structures.

     
  3. 3.

    Strong competence hypothesis: The principles of the competence grammar are directly used by the human language processor in constructing a syntactic structure and interpreting it.

     

Keywords

Noun Phrase Syntactic Structure Parse Tree Phrase Structure Rule Syntactic Constituent 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. Abney, S.: 1989, ‘A Computational Model of Human Parsing’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 18, 129–144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Aho, A. and J. Ullman: 1977, Principles of Compiler Design, Addison-Wesley, Menlo Park, California.Google Scholar
  3. Berwick, R. and A. Weinberg: 1983, ‘The Role of Grammars in Models of Language Use’, Cognition 13, 1–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  4. Berwick, R. and A. Weinberg: 1984, The Grammatical Basis of Linguistic Performance: Language Use and Acquisition, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  5. Berwick, R. and A. Weinberg: 1985a, ‘Deterministic Parsing and Linguistic Explanation’, Language and Cognitive Processes 1, 109–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  6. Berwick, R. and A. Weinberg: 1985b, ‘The Psychological Relevance of Transformational Grammar: A Reply to Stabler’, Cognition 19, 193–204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  7. Bobrow, R. and B. Webber: 1980, ‘Knowledge Representation for Syntactic/Semantic Processing’, Proceedings of the First Annual Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Morgan-Kaufmann Publishers, Los Altos, California, pp. 316–323.Google Scholar
  8. Bresnan, J. and R. Kaplan: 1982, ‘Introduction: Grammars as Mental Representations of Language’, in J. Bresnan (ed.), The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. xvii–1ii.Google Scholar
  9. Church, K. and R. Patil: 1982, ‘Coping with Syntactic Ambiguity’, American Journal of Computational Linguistics 8, 139–149.Google Scholar
  10. Clifton, C. and L. Frazier: 1986, ‘The Use of Syntactic Information in Filling Gaps’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 15, 209–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  11. Fodor, J.D.: 1988, ‘On Modularity in Syntactic Processing’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 17, 125–168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Fodor, J.D.: 1991 forthcoming, ‘Sentence Processing and Mental Grammar’, in P. Sells, S. Shieber, and T. Wasow (eds.), Foundational Issues in Natural Language Processing, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  13. Frazier, L., C. Clifton, and J. Randall: 1983, ‘Filling Gaps: Decision Principles and Structure in Sentence Comprehension’, Cognition 13, 187–222.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. Keenan, E.: 1989, ‘Semantic Case Theory’, In R. Bartsch, J. van Bentham, and R. van Emde-Boas (eds.), Semantics and Contextual Expression, Dordrecht, Foris, Groningen-Amsterdam Studies in Semantics (GRASS) Volume 11, pp. 33–57.Google Scholar
  15. Keenan, E. and L. Faltz: 1985, Boolean Semantics for Natural Language, Reidel, Boston, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  16. Knuth, D.: 1973, The Art of Computer Programming, Volume 1: Fundamental Algorithms, Addison-Wesley, Reading, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  17. Kowalski, R.: 1979, Logic for Problem Solving, North-Holland, New York.Google Scholar
  18. Kowalski, R. and D. Kuehner: 1971, ‘Linear Resolution with Selection Functions’, Artificial Intelligence 2, 227–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Langendoen, T., A. Koslow, and C. Neff: 1988, ‘The Syntax and Semantics of Coordinate Compounding in English’, unpublished manuscript, City University of New York, New York.Google Scholar
  20. Lloyd, J.: 1987, Foundations of Logic Programming, Second Edition Springer-Verlag, New York.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Loveland, D.: 1972, ‘A Unifying View of Some Linear Herbrand Procedures’, Journal of the Association for Computing Machinery 19, 366–384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Marslen-Wilson, W. and L.K. Tyler: 1980, ‘The Temporal Structure of Spoken Language Understanding’, Cognition 8, 1–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Marslen-Wilson, W.: 1987, ‘Functional Parallelism and Spoken Word Recognition’, Cognition 25, 71–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Marslen-Wilson, W.: 1989, ‘Access and Integration: Projecting Sound onto Meaning’, in W. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical Representation and Process, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 3–24.Google Scholar
  25. Marslen-Wilson, W.: 1989, (ed.), Lexical Representation and Process, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  26. Matthews, R.: 1988, ‘Psychological Reality of Grammars’, Unpublished paper presented at the conference on The Chomskyan Turn, Tel-Aviv and Jerusalem, April, 1988.Google Scholar
  27. Naish, L.: 1986, Negation and Control in Prolog, Springer-Verlag, New York.Google Scholar
  28. Pereira, F. and D.H.D. Warren: 1980, ‘Definite Clause Grammars for Natural Language Analysis’, Artificial Intelligence 13, 231–278.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Quine, W.V.: 1961, ‘The Ways of Paradox’, in W.V. Quine, The Ways of Paradox and other Essays, Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 1–18.Google Scholar
  30. Reiter, R.: 1971, ‘Two Results on Ordering for Resolution With Merging and Linear Format’, Journal of the Association for Computing Machinery 18, 630–646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Stabler, E.P. Jr.: 1983, ‘How Are Grammars Represented?’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6, 391–421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Stabler, E.P. Jr.: 1984, ‘Berwick and Weinberg on Linguistics and Cognitive Psychology’, Cognition 17, 155–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Stabler, E.P. Jr.: 1990, ‘Representing Knowledge with Theories about Theories’, Journal of Logic Programming 9, 105–138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Stabler, E.P. Jr.: 1991 forthcoming, The Logical Approach to Syntax: Foundations, Specifications, and Implementations of Theories of Government and Binding, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts.Google Scholar
  35. Steedman, M.: 1989, ‘Grammar, Interpretation and Processing From the Lexicon’, in W. Marslen-Wilson (ed.), Lexical Representation and Process, MIT Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, pp. 463–504.Google Scholar
  36. Tyler, L.: 1980, Serial and Interactive Parallel Theories of Syntactic Processing, Occasional Paper No. 8, Cambridge, Massachusetts, MIT Center for Cognitive Science.Google Scholar
  37. Tyler, L. and W. Marslen-Wilson: 1977, ‘The On-line Effects of Semantic Context on Syntactic Processing’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16, 683–692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. VanderBrug, G. and J. Minker: 1975, ‘State-space, Problem-reduction, and Theorem-proving—Some Relationships’, Communications of the Association for Computing Machinery 18, 107–115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Winograd, T.: 1972, Understanding Natural Language, Academic Press, New York.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 1991

Authors and Affiliations

  • Edward P. StablerJr.
    • 1
  1. 1.Department of LinguisticsUniversity of California at Los AngelesLos AngelesUSA

Personalised recommendations