Skip to main content

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 84))

  • 212 Accesses

Abstract

Highly competent people have more success in life, both actively because they are able to act in ways which secure attainment of their goals, and passively, because they are attractive to other people. Typically, highly competent people are also proud of themselves.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Atkinson, J.W. (1957). otivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64, 359–372.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P.B., & Baltes, M.M. (Eds.), (1990). Successful aging. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baltes, P.B., Smith, J., Staudinger, U.M., & Sowarka, D. (1990). Wisdom: One facet of successful aging? In M. Perlmutter (Ed.), Late Life Potential (pp. 63–81). Washington, D.C.: The Gerontological Society of America.

    Google Scholar 

  • Band, E.B., & Weisz, J.R. (1988). How to feel better when it feels bad: Children’s perspectives on coping with everyday stress. Developmental Psychology, 24, 247–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84, 191–215.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1994). Self-Efficacy. The Exercise of Control. New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bivens, J.A., & Berk, L.E. (1990). A longitudinal study of the development of elementary school children’s private speech. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 36, 443–463.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blumenfeld, P., Pintrich, P., & Hamilton, V. (1986). Children’s concept of ability, effort, and conduct. American Educational Research Journal, 23, 95–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Boggiano, A.K., Main, D.S., & Katz, P.A. (1988). Children’s preference for challenge: The role of perceived competence and control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 134–141.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., & Renner, G. (1990). Tenacious goal pursuit and flexible goal adjustment: Explication and age-related analysis of assimilative and accommodative strategies of coping. Psychology and Aging, 5, 58–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brandtstädter, J., Wentura, D, & Greve, W. (1993). Adaptive resources of the aging self: Outlines of an emergent perspective. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 323–349.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. (1983). Child’s Talk: Learning to use Language. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bühler, K. (1919). Abriss der geistigen Entwicklung des Kindes. Leipzig: Quelle & Meyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bullock, M., Lütkenhaus, P. (1988). The development of volitional behavior in the toddler years. Child Development, 59, 664–674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, L.B., & Oakes, L.M. (1993). How infants perceive a simple causal event. Developmental Psychology, 29, 421–433.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diener, E. (1984). Subjective well-being. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 542–575.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elkind, D. (1967). Egocentrism in adolescence. Child Development, 38, 1025–1034.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Erikson, E.H. (1968). Identity. Youth and Crisis. New York: Norton (dt. Jugend und Krise. Weinheim: Klett-Cotta, 1981).

    Google Scholar 

  • Fend, H. (1991). Identitätsentwicklung in der Adoleszenz. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Finkelstein, N.W., & Ramey, C. (1977). Learning to control the environment in infancy. Child Development, 48, 806–819.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flammer, A. (1988). Entwicklungstheorien. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flammer, A. (1990). Erfahrung der eigenen Wirksamkeit. Bern: Huber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flammer, A. (1992). Secondary control in an individual-centered and in a group-centered culture. In W. Meeus, M. De Goede, W. Kox, & K. Hurreimann (Eds.), Adolescence, Careers, and Cultures (pp. 131–141). Berlin: De Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flammer, A., Grob, A., Lüthi, R., & Kaiser, F.G. (1989). Kontrollattributionen und Wohlbefinden von Schweizer Jugendlichen. Research Report Nr. 1989-4. University of Berne, Switzerland: Department of Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flammer, A., Züblin, C., & Grob, A. (1988). Sekundäre Kontrolle bei Jugendlichen. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 20, 239–262.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flammer, A. (1994). Developmental analysis of control beliefs. In A. Bandura (Ed.). Self-Efficacy in Changing Societies. New York: Cambridge University Press, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frieze, I.H., & Snyder, H.L. (1980). Children’s belief about the causes of success and failure in school settings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 186–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fry, P.S. (1975). Affect and resistance to temptation. Developmental Psychology, 11, 466–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geppert, U., & Küster, U. (1983). The emergence of “wanting to do it oneself:” A precursor of achievement motivation. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 3, 355–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M.R. (1980a). Contingent stimulation: A review of its role in early development. In S. Levine, & H. Ursin (Eds.), Coping and Health (pp. 101–119). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., Solomon, S., Pyszczynski, T., Rosenblatt, A., Burling, J., Lyon, D., Simon, L., & Pinel, E. (1992). Why do people need self-esteem? Converging evidence that self-esteem serves an anxiety-buffering function. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63, 913–922.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grob, A., Flammer, A., Kaiser, F., Lüthi, R. (1989). Wohlbefinden und Kontrolle bei jugendlichen Delinquenten und Nicht-Delinquenten. Schweizerische Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 48, 75–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar, M.R. (1980b). Control, warning signals, and distress in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 16, 281–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnar-VonGnechten, M.R. (1978). Changing a frightening toy into a pleasant toy by allowing the infant to control its action. Developmental Psychology, 14, 157–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Halisch, C., & Halisch, F. (1980). Kognitive Voraussetzungen frühkindlicher Selbstbewertungsreaktionen nach Erfolg und Misserfolg. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 12, 193–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, K.R. (1990). Developing self-regulated learners: The role of private speech and self-instructions. Special Issue: Self-regulated learning and academic achievement. Educational Psychologist, 25, 35–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1966). Die Entwicklung des Erlebens von Erfolg und Misserfolg. Bild der Wissenschaft, 7, 547–553 (Nachdruck in C.F. Graumann, & H. Heckhausen (Hg.), Pädagogische Psychologie 1. Frankfurt: Fischer, 1973, 95–105).

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1982). The development of achievement motivation. In W.W. Hartup (Ed.), Review of Child Development Research (Vol. 6, pp. 600–668). Chicago, IL.: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, H. (1984). Emergent achievement behavior: Some early developments. In J.G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (Vol. 3, pp. 1–37). Greenwich, Ct.: JAI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J. (1988). Becoming aware of one’s competence in the second year: Developmental progression within the mother-child dyad. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 305–326.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heckhausen, J., & Schulz, R. (in press). A life-span theory of control. Psychological Review.

    Google Scholar 

  • Helmke, A. (1993). Achievement-related motives, self-evaluations, causal attributions and school experiences of third graders. In F.E. Weinert, & W. Schneider (Eds.), The Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC), Report No. 9 (pp. 111–133). Munich: Max-Planck-Institute for Psychological Research.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hetzer, H. (1931). Kind und Schaffen. Jena: Gustav Fischer.

    Google Scholar 

  • J. Rodin, C. Schooler, & K.W. Schaie (Eds.), Self-Directedness: Cause and Effects throughout the Life Course (pp. 213–229). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hohmann, P.M. (1988). Kontrolle und Zufriedenheit in Beziehungen. München: Profil.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karabenick, J., & Heller, K. (1976). A developmental study of effort and ability attributions. Developmental Psychology, 12, 559–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kun, A. (1977). Development of magnitude-covariation and compensation schemata in ability and effort attributions of performance. Child Development, 48, 862–873.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kun, A., Parsons, J., & Ruble, D. (1974). Development of integration processes using ability and effort information to predict outcome. Developmental Psychology, 10, 721–732.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kunnen, S. (1993). Attributions and perceived control over school failure in handicapped and non-handicapped children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 16, 113–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Langer, E.J. (1975). The illusion of control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 32, 311–328.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A.M. (1982). The perception of causality in infants. Perception, 11, 173–186.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A.M. (1984). Spatiotemporal continuity and the perception of causality in infants. Perception, 13, 287–305.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A.M., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-olds perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265–288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A.R. (1961). The role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. New York: Basic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A.R. (1976). Cognitive development. Its cultural and social foundations. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lütkenhaus, P., Bullock, M., & Geppert, U. (1987). Toddler’s action: Knowledge, control and the self. In F. Halisch, & J. Kuhl (Eds.), Motivation, Intention, and Volition (pp. 145–161). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • MacFarlane, J. (1975). Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate. In M. Hofer (Ed.), Parent-Infant Interaction (pp. 103–117). Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mandler, J.M. (1992). How to build a baby: II. Conceptual primitives. Psychological Review, 99, 587–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michotte, A. (1963). The Perception of Causality. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, A. (1985). A developmental study of the cognitive basis of performance impairment after failure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 529–538.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, H.N., & Mischel, W. (1983). The development of children’s knowledge of self-control strategies. Child Development, 54, 603–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., & Rodriguez, M.L. (1989). Delay of gratification in children. Science, 244, 933–938.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, A. (1958). Ueber die Entwicklung des Leistungsanspruchsniveaus. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 162, 238–353.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J.G. (1978). The development of the concepts of effort and ability, perception of academic attainment, and the understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J.G. (1980). The development of the concept of difficulty. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 26, 271–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J.G., & Miller, A.T. (1984). Development and its discontents: The differentiation of the concept of ability. In J.G. Nicholls (Ed.), Advances in Motivation and Achievement, Vol. 3: The Development of Achievement Motivation (pp. 185–218). Greenwich, Ct.: JAI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J.G., & Miller, A.T. (1985). Differentiation of the concepts of luck and skill. Developmental Psychology, 21, 76–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1979). Early ontogeny of human social interaction: Its biological roots and social dimensions. In M. Von Cranach, K. Foppa, W. Lepenies, & D. Ploog (Eds.), Human Ethology: Claims and Limits of a New Discipline (pp. 456–478). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Papousek, H., & Papousek, M. (1989). Stimmliche Kommunikation im frühen Säuglingsalter als Wegbereiter der Sprachentwicklung. In H. Keller (Hg.), Handbuch der Kleinkindforschung (pp. 465–490). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Patterson, C.J., & Mischel, W. (1976). Effects of temptation-inhibiting and task-facilitating plans on self-control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 209–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1936). La Naissance de l’Intelligence chez l’Enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1937). La Construction du Réel chez l’Enfant. Neuchâtel: Delachaux et Niestlé.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramey, C.T., & Finkelstein, N.W. (1978). Contingent stimulation and infant competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 3, 89–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rholes, W., Blackwell, J., Jordan, C., & Walters, C. (1980). A developmental study of learned helplessness. Developmental Psychology, 16, 616–624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riksen-Walraven, J.M. (1978). Effects of caregiver behavior on habituation rate and self-efficacy in infants. Internationaljournal of Behavioral Development, 1, 105–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rothbaum, F.M., Weisz, J.R., & Snyder, S.S. (1982). Changing the world and changing the self: A two-process model of perceived control. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42, 5–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruble, D.N., & Flett, G.L. (1988). Conflicting goals in self-evaluative information seeking: Developmental and ability level analyses. Child Development, 59, 97–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ryff, C.D. (1989). In the eye of the beholder: Views of psychological well-being among middle-aged and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 4, 195–210. bestt.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P. (1975). Helplessness. On Depression, Development, and Death. San Francisco: Freeman (dt. Erlernte Hilflosigkeit. München: Urban & Schwarzenberg, 1983).

    Google Scholar 

  • Shoda, Y., Mischel, W., & Peake, P.K. (1990). Predicting adolescent cognitive and self-regulatory competencies from preschool delay of gratification: Identifying diagnostic conditions. Developmental Psychology, 26, 978–986.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siqueland, E.R., & Delucia, C.A. (1969). Visual reinforcement of non-nutritive sucking in human infants. Science, 165, 1144–1146.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siqueland, E.R., & Lippsitt, L.P. (1966). Conditioned head-turning behavior in newborns. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 3, 356–376.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E.A., & Chapman, M. (1984). Control beliefs in an action perspective. Human Development, 77, 129–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E.A., Chapman, M., & Baltes, P.B. (1988a). Children’s belief about control, means-ends, and agency: Developmental differences during middle childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 11, 369–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, E.A., Chapman, M., & Baltes, P.B. (1988b). Control, means-ends, and agency beliefs: A new conceptualization and its measurement during childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54, 117–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D.N. (1985). The Interpersonal World of the Infants. New York: Basic (German edition: Die Lebenserfahrung des Säuglings. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta, 1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Stiensmeier, J. (1986). Wichtigkeit und Kontrollerleben als Bedingungen von Lageorientierung. Archiv für Psychologie, 138, 127–138.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. (1981). Children’s perceptions of their own and their class-mates abilities. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 404–410.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. (1983). A developmental analysis of pride and shame. Human Development, 26, 42–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D., & Tannati, L. (1984). Children’s judgments of their own and their peers’ academic competence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 75–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Strube, M.J., Lott, C.L., Lê-Xuân-Hy, G.M., Oxenberg, J., & Deichman, A.K. (1986). Self-evaluation of abilities: Accurate self-assessment versus biased self-enhancement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 16–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suomi, S.J. (1981). The perception of contingency and social development. In M.E. Lamb, & L.R. Sherrod (Eds.), Infant Social Cognition. Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum, 177–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trope, Y. (1982). Self-assessment and task performance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 18, 201–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L.S. (1934, transi. 1962). Thought and Language. Cambridge, MA: Masachusetts Institute of Technology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.S. (1966). The development and generalization of “contingency awareness” in early infancy: Some hypotheses. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 12, 123–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.S. (1967). Memory and “contingent analysis” in infant learning. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 13, 55–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.S. (1971). Cognitive perceptual development in infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarerly, 18, 139–152.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.S. (1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In E.B. Thoman (Ed.), Origins of the infant’s social responsiveness (pp. 33–64). Hillsdale, N.J.: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Watson, J.S., & Ramey, C.T. (1972). Reactions to response-contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 18, 219–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, B., Frieze, I.H., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R.M. (1971). Perceiving the Causes of Success and Failure. New York: General Learning Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J.R. (1980). Developmental change in perceived control: Recognizing noncontingency in the lavoratory and perceiving it in the world. Developmental Psychology, 16, 385–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J.R. (1981). Illusory contingency in children at the state fair. Developmental Psychology, 17, 481–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weisz, J.R., Rothbaum, F.M., & Blackburn, T.C. (1984). Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in America and Japan. American Psychologist, 39, 955–969.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yussen, S., & Kane, P. (1985). Children’s conception of intelligence. In S.W. Yussen (Ed.), The growth of Reflection in Children (pp. 207–241). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1995 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Flammer, A. (1995). Possum, ergo Sum—Nequeo, ergo Sum qui Sum. In: Oosterwegel, A., Wicklund, R.A. (eds) The Self in European and North American Culture: Development and Processes. NATO ASI Series, vol 84. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-0331-2_24

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-011-0331-2_24

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-4146-1

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-011-0331-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics