Skip to main content

Spatial Transformations used in Imagination, Perception and Action

  • Chapter
Matters of Intelligence

Part of the book series: Synthese Library ((SYLI,volume 188))

  • 387 Accesses

Abstract

The work described in this chapter was originally motivated by an interest in understanding a fundamental unsolved problem in human perception—how we recognize an object, in view of the fact that there is an ambiguous and indirect mapping between information in the array of light intensities at the retina and the object. Movement of the observer’s eyes or body or of the object affect in highly variable ways the image of the object projected on the retina. The approach taken to this problem was to investigate the theoretical and psychophysical aspects of the ability to discriminate between identical or mostly similar objects at different orientations. As discussed below, people often find this discrimination too difficult and “correct” for the difference in the orientation of the objects by either imagining or physically producing a compensatory spatial transformation to align the objects with respect to either a scene-based or egocentric frame of reference. What happens is people do some initial perceptual processing of the objects, then produce some action or mental simulation of action, and then do more perceptual processing to form an internal representation of the object sufficiently detailed to allow a discrimination. So information and processes related to action interact (cyclically) with information and processes related to the perception of object shape. Although this kind of information processing has recently been the subject of theoretical concern,1 I will mostly discuss recent empirical findings.2

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  1. D. Malt and L. Vaina, “Representation and recognition of the movement of shapes,” Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B, 204, 301–328, 1982

    Google Scholar 

  2. V. Neisser, Cognition and Reality, W. H. Freman and Co., San Francisco, 1976

    Google Scholar 

  3. R. N. Shepard, “Ecological constraints on internal representation: resonant kinematics of perceiving, imagining, thinking and dreaming,” Psychol. Rev., 91, 417–447, 1984.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. L. M. Parsons, “Mental rotation paths in the discrimination of left and right parts of the body,” unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, San Diego, 1983a

    Google Scholar 

  5. L. M. Parsons, “Imagined spatial transformations in the discrimination of left and right parts of the human body,” Proc. 5th Annual Conference Cognitive Science Soc., Rochester, New York, 1983b

    Google Scholar 

  6. L. M. Parsons, “Imagined spatial transformations of one’s hand and foot,” submitted for publication, 1986c

    Google Scholar 

  7. L. M. Parsons, “Imagined spatial transformations of one’s body,” submitted for publication, 1986b

    Google Scholar 

  8. L. M. Parsons, “Visual discrimination of abstract mirror-reversed three-dimensional objects at many orientation,” submitted for publication, 1986f.

    Google Scholar 

  9. E. C. Hildreth and J. M. Hollerbach, “The computational approach to vision and motor control,” Artificial Intelligence Laboratory Memo 846, A. I. Laboratory, M.I. T., Cambridge, 1985

    Google Scholar 

  10. D. Marr and T. Poggio, “From understanding computation to under standing neural circuitry,” Neurosci. Res. Prog. Bull., 15, 470–488, 1977.

    Google Scholar 

  11. L. M. Parsons, “Evaluation and use of spatial transformation procedures in apparent motion and imagination,” submitted for publication, 1986a

    Google Scholar 

  12. L. Parsons, 1986c.

    Google Scholar 

  13. M. A. Just and P. A. Carpenter, “Cognitive coordinate systems: accounts of mental rotation and individual differences in spatial ability,” Psychological Rev., 92, 137–172, 1985

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. L. M. Parsons. “Evaluation and use of spatial transformation procedures in apparent motion and imagination,” submitted for publication, 1986a

    Google Scholar 

  15. L. M. Parsons, 1986b; L. M. Parsons, 1986c

    Google Scholar 

  16. L. M. Parsons, “Procedures for computing trajectories between different orientations and positions of an object,” unpublished manuscript, 1986d

    Google Scholar 

  17. L. M. Parsons, “Properties of reasoned or indicated trajectories between different orientations and positions of an object,” unpublished manuscript, 1986e

    Google Scholar 

  18. L. M. Parsons, “Visual discrimination of abstract mirror-reflected three-dimensional objects at many orientations,” submitted for publication, 1986f.

    Google Scholar 

  19. W. Abend, E. Bizzi and P. Morasso, “Human arm trajectory formation,” Brain, 105, 331–348, 1982

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. T. Flash, “Organizing principles underlying the formation of hand trajectories,” unpublished doctoral dissertation, Harvard/M.I. T. Division of Health Sciences and Technology, 1983

    Google Scholar 

  21. J. M. Hollerbach and T. Flash, “Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm movement,” Bioi. Cyber., 44, 67–77, 1982

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. P. Morasso, “Spatial control of arm movements,” Exp. Brain Res., 42, 223–227, 1981

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. J. F. Soechting and F. Lacquaniti, “Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man,” J. Neuroscience, 1, 710–720, 1981

    Google Scholar 

  24. J. F. Soechting, “Does position sense at the elbow reflect a sense of elbow joint angle or of limb orientation?” Brain Res., 248, 392–395, 1982

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. J. F. Soechting, F. Lacquaniti and C. A. Terzuolo, “Coordination of arm movements in three-dimensional space: sensorimotor mapping during drawing movement,” Neurosci., 17, 295–311

    Google Scholar 

  26. J. F. Soechting and B. Ross, “Psychophysical determination of coordinate representation of human arm orientation,” Neurosci., 5, 1085–1103, 1984.

    Google Scholar 

  27. W. R. Garner, The processing of information and structure, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., 1974

    Google Scholar 

  28. S. E. Palmer, “Symmetry, transformation and the structure of perceptual systems,” In: Organization and representation in perception, J. Beck, ed., Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., 1982

    Google Scholar 

  29. R. N. Shepard, “Connections between the representation of shapes and their spatial transformation,” In: Representations of three-dimensional objects, Springer-Verlag, Berlin, in press.

    Google Scholar 

  30. R. N. Shepard and L. A. Cooper, Mental images and their transformations, MIT Press/Bradford Books, Cambridge, Mass., 1982.

    Google Scholar 

  31. L. M. Parsons and G. E. Hinton, “Egocentric and environmental frames of reference in the discrimination of mirror-image shapes,” manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  32. L. A. Cooper and R. N. Shepard, “Mental transformations in the identification of left and right hands,” J. Exp. Psych: Human Perception and Performance, 104, 48–56, 1975

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. S. Pinker, “Mental imagery and the third dimension,” J. Exp. Psych.: General, 109, 254–371, 1980.

    Google Scholar 

  34. J. Metzler, “Cognitive analogues of the rotation of three-dimensional objects,” unpublished doctoral dissertation, Stanford University, 1973

    Google Scholar 

  35. L. A. Cooper, “Demonstration of a mental analog of an external rotation,” Perception and Psychophysics, 19, 433–444, 1976.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. I. Rock “perceptual uprightness” in his Orientation and form, Academic Press, New York, 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  37. See L. Parsons, 1986a, L. Parsons, 1986d.

    Google Scholar 

  38. L. Parsons, 1983a.

    Google Scholar 

  39. L. ParsoIls, 1986a, 1986d.

    Google Scholar 

  40. The exceptions are Just and Carpenter, 1985

    Google Scholar 

  41. L. Parsons, 1983a, 1983b; also Cooper and Shepard, 1975

    Google Scholar 

  42. Y. Sayeki, “ ‘Body analogy’ and the cognition of rotated figures,” Q. Newsletter of Lab. Compar. Human Cog.., 3, 36–40, 1981

    Google Scholar 

  43. K. Sekiyama, “Kinesthetic aspects of mental representations in the identification of left and right hands,” Percept. and Psychophys., 32, 89–95, 1982

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. L. Parsons, 1986c

    Google Scholar 

  45. L. Parsons, “Representing and processing spatial information about connected sensory and motor surfaces,” unpublished manuscript.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Hochberg, 1981

    Google Scholar 

  47. Neisser, 1976

    Google Scholar 

  48. Parsons, 1986c

    Google Scholar 

  49. Shepard, 1984.

    Google Scholar 

  50. W. L. Nelson, “Physical principles for economies of skilled movements,” BioI. Cyber., 46, 135–147, 1983.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. J. deKleer and J. S. Brown, “Mental models of physical mechanisms and their acquisition,” In: Cognitive skills and their acquisition, Erlbaum, Hillside, N.J., 1976

    Google Scholar 

  52. D. Gentner and A. L. Stevens, Mental models, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, N.J., 1983.

    Google Scholar 

  53. L. Parsons, 1986b.

    Google Scholar 

  54. This is confirmed by L. Parsons, 1986b.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Parsons, 1986c.

    Google Scholar 

  56. L. Parsons, 1986e

    Google Scholar 

  57. L. Parsons, 1986b, c, e, f.

    Google Scholar 

  58. R. N. Shepard, “Form, formation and transformation of internal representations,” in: Information processiftg and cognition: the Loyola Symposium, R. Solso, Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  59. G. E. Hinton, “Parallel computations for controlling an arm,” J. Motor Behavior, 16, 171–194, 1984

    Google Scholar 

  60. J. M. Hollerbach and C. Atkeson, “Characterization of jointinterpolated arm movements,” MIT Artificial Intelligence Laboratory Memo 849, 1985

    Google Scholar 

  61. D. McDermott and E. Davis, “Planning routes through uncertain territory,” Artificial Intelligence, 22, 107–156, 1984

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. T. Lozano-Perez, “Spatial planning: a configuration approach,” IEEE Trans. on Computers, 32, 108–120, 1983

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. L. Parsons, 1986a; 1986d

    Google Scholar 

  64. L. Parsons, “Representing and processing spatial information about connected sensory and motor surfaces,” unpublished manuscript; Shepard, in press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1987 D. Reidel Publishing Company, Dordrecht, Holland

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Parsons, L.M. (1987). Spatial Transformations used in Imagination, Perception and Action. In: Vaina, L.M. (eds) Matters of Intelligence. Synthese Library, vol 188. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-3833-5_6

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-3833-5_6

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-8206-8

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-009-3833-5

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics