Skip to main content

The Instrumental Effects of Emotional Behavior — Consequences for the Physiological State

  • Chapter
Cognitive Perspectives on Emotion and Motivation

Part of the book series: NATO ASI Series ((ASID,volume 44))

Abstract

Aggressive and fear types of behavior are usually executed under conditions of high physiological activation, and are referred to as emotions. But the same type of behavior may also be associated with moderate or even low levels of activation. This chapter deals with the psychological mechanisms that determine which internal state a subject is likely to be in during execution of a particular type of emotional behavior (fear behavior, aggressive behavior).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 259.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 329.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anisman, H. (1978). Neurochemical changes elicited by stress. Behavioral correlates. In H. Anisman & G. Bignami, Psychopharmacology of aversively motivated behavior. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bandura, A. (1987). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A.T. (1967). Depression: Clinical, experimental and theoretical aspects. New York: Hoeber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger, H. (1930). Uber das Elektroenkephalogramm des Menschen II. Journal of Physiology and Neurology, 40, 160–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blaney, P.H. (1977). Comtemporary theories of depression: Critique and comparison. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 86, 203–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bolles, R.C. (1972). Reinforcement, expectancy and learning. Psychological Review, 79, 394–409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brodal, A. (1957). The reticular formation of the brain stem. Anatomical aspects and functional correlations. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coover, G.D., Ursin, H., & Levine, S. (1973). Plasma-corticosterone levels during active-avoidance learning in rats. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 82, 170–174.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coover, G.D., Ursin, H., & Murison, R. (1983). Sustained activation and psychiatric illness. In H. Ursin & R. Murison, Biological and psychological basis of psychosomatic disease. Oxford: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coyne, J.C. (1976). Toward an interactional description of depression. Psychiatry, 39, 28–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dess, N.K., Linwick, D., Patterson, J., Overmier, J.B., & Levine, S. (1983). Immediate and proactive effects of controllability and predictability on plasma Cortisol responses to shocks in dogs. Behavioral Neuroscience, 97, 1005–1016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, A. (1980). Contemporary animal learning theory. Oxford: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erickson, R.C., Post, R.D., & Paige, A.B. (1975). Hope as a psychiatric variable. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 31, 324–330.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Edmunds, M. (1974). Defense in animals: A survey of antipredator defenses. New York: Longman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feshbach, S. (1964). The function of aggression and the regulation of aggressive drive. Psychological Review, 71, 257–272.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Folkman, S. (1984). Personal control and stress and coping processes: A theoretical analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 838–852.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Forrest, M.S., & Hokanson, J.E. (1975). Depression and autonomic arousal reduction accompanying self-punitive behavior. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 84, 346–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frankenhaeuser, M. (1975). Experimental approaches to the study of catecholamines and emotion. In L. Levi, Emotions: Their parameters and measurement. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freud, S. (1975). New introductory lectures. London: Hogarth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J.A. (1975). Elements of a two-process theory of learning. London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haan, N. (1978). Coping and defending. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, V. (1983). The cognitive structures and processes of human motivation personality. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, V., & Warburton, D.M. (1979). Human stress and Cognition: An information processing approach. (New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendrick, I. (1943). The discussion of the “instinct to master”. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 12, 561–565.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hiroto, D.S., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1975). Generality of learned helplessness in man. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 311–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hull, C.H. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York, Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, J.A., & Brazier, M.A.B. (1980). The reticular formation revisited: Specifying function for a nonspecific system. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Irwin, F.W. (1971). Intentional behavior and motivation. Philadelphia: J.B. Lippincott.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H.H. (1963). Studies of non-specific effects upon electrical responses in sensory systems. In G. Moruzzi, A. Fessard & H.H. Jasper (Eds.), Brain mechanisms. Progress in Brain Research, Vol. 1.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jones, B.E. (1981). Understanding the physiological correlates of behavioral state as a constellation of events. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 482–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Konorski, J. (1981). Integrative activity of the brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kragh, U. (1960). The defense mechanism test: A new method for diagnosis and personnel selection. Journal of Applied Psychology, 44, 303–309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhl, J. (1988). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P.J. (1988). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S. (1966). Psychological stress and the coping process. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lazarus, R.S. (1982). Thoughts on the relations between emotion and cognition. American Psychologists, 37, 1019–1024.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levine, S., Weinberg, J., & Ursin, H. (1978). Definition of the coping process and statement of the problem. In H. Ursin, E. Baade & S. Levine (Eds.) Psychobiology of stress: A study of coping men. New York, Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, D.B. (1951). Emotion. In S. Stevens (Ed.), Handbook of experimental psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malone, J.C. (1975). The “paradigms” of learning. The Psychological Record, 25, 479–489.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mason, J.W. (1971). A re-evaluation of the concept of “non-specificity” in stress theory. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 8, 323–335.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Masserman, J.H. (1943). Behavior and neurosis. Chicago, University of Chicago.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G.A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K.H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mineka, S., & Kihlstrom, J.F. (1978). Unpredictable and uncontrollable events: A new perspective on experimental neurosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 87, 256–271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moos, R.H., & Billings, A.G. (1982). Conceptualizing and measuring coping resources and processes. In Goldberger, L. & Breznitz, S. (Eds.), Handbook of stress. Theoretical and clinical aspects. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Monat, A. Averill, J.R., & Lazarus, R.S. (1972). Anticipatory stress and coping reactions under various conditions of uncertainty. Journal of Personalilty and Social Psychology, 24, 237–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O.H. (1960). Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O.H., & Viek, P. (1948). An experimental analogue of fear from a sense of helplessness. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 43, 193–200.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moruzzi G., & Magoun, H.W. (1949). Brain stem reticular formation and activation of the EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 1, 455–473.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Olweus, D. (1978). Aggression in the schools. Hemisphere: Washington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ortony, F. (1988). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J.B., Patterson, I., & Wielkiewic, R.M. (1980). Environmental contingencies as sources of stress in animals. In S. Levine & H. Ursin (Eds.), Coping and Health. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, J.B. & Seligman, M.E.P. (1967). Effect of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 28–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Overmier, B., & Seligman, M.E.P. (1967). Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance learning. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 63, 28–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlov, I.P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pearlin, L.I., Lieberman, M.A., Menaghan, E.G., & Mullen, J.T. (1981). The stress process. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337–356.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pervin, L.A. (1963). The need to predict and control under conditions of threat. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 570–585.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D. (1959). Toward empirical behavior laws: I. Positive reinforcement. Psychological Review, 66, 219–233.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pribram, J.H., & Melges, F.T. (1969). Psychophysiological basis of emotion. In P.J. Vinken & G.W. Bruyn(Eds.), Handbook of clinical neurology, Vol. 3. New York: J. Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prociuk, T.J., Breen, L.J., & Lussier, R.J. (1976). Hopelessness, internal-external locus of control and depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 299–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ranck, J.B. (1981). An obituary for old arousal theory. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 487–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R.A., & Solomon, R.L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian conditioning and instrumental learning. Psychological Review, 74, 151–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Risberg, J. (1980). Regional cerebral blood flow measurements by 133Xe-inhalation: Methodology and applications in neurophysiology and psychiatry. Brain and Language, 9, 9–34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rotter, J.B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal control of reinforcement. Psychological Monograph, 80, 609.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seligman, M.E.P., Maier, S.F., & Solomon, R.L. (1971). Unpredictable and uncontrollable aversive events. In F.R. Brush, Aversive conditioning and learning. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents. Nature, 138, 32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steriade, M. (1981). EEG desynchronization is associated with cellular events that are prereginisites for active behavioral states. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 489–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toates F.M. (1988). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tolman, E.C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men. Century: New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ursin, H. (1980). Personality, activation and somatic health. A new psychosomatic theory. In S. Levine & H. Ursin (Eds.), Coping and health. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ursin, H. (1980). The instrumental effects of emotional behavior. In P.P.G. Bateson & P.H. Klopfer (Eds.), Perspectives in ethology. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ursin, H., Baade, E., & Levine, S. (1978). Psychobiology of stress: A study of coping men. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vanderwolf, C.H., & Robinson, T.E. (1981). Reticulo-cortical activity and behavior: A critique of the arousal theory and a new synthesis. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 459–514.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Værnes, R.J. (1983). Defensive reactions in dangerous occupations. In H. Ursin & R. Murison (Eds.), Biological and psychological basis of psychosomatic disease. Pergamon: Oxford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warburton, D.M. (1988). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinberg, J., & Levine, S. (1980). Psychobiology of coping in animals: The effects of predictability. In S. Levine & H. Ursin (Eds.), Coping and health. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiss, J.M., Bailey, W.H., Goodman, P.A., Hoffman, L.J., Ambrose, M.J., Salman, S., & Charry, J.M. (1982). A model for neurochemical study of depression. In M.Y. Spiegelstein & A. Levy (Eds.), Behavioral models and the analysis of drug action. Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, R.W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Øhman, A. (1988). This volume.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1988 Kluwer Academic Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ursin, H. (1988). The Instrumental Effects of Emotional Behavior — Consequences for the Physiological State. In: Hamilton, V., Bower, G.H., Frijda, N.H. (eds) Cognitive Perspectives on Emotion and Motivation. NATO ASI Series, vol 44. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-2792-6_9

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-2792-6_9

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-94-010-7756-9

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-009-2792-6

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics