Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 7))

Abstract

In this chapter we explore a controversial hypothesis about the nature of the syntactic processing system. The hypothesis, roughly stated, is that some of the procedures that create grammatical patterns in sentences are in an important sense indifferent to the content of the symbols they manipulate, in somewhat the same way that the procedures for long multiplication are indifferent to the numbers involved in the computation. The purpose of this exploration is to uncover the linguistic or cognitive bases of a phenomenon noted by many, including Edward Sapir, who described it in this way:

All languages evince a curious instinct for the development of one or more particular grammatical processes at the expense of others, tending always to lose sight of any explicit functional value that the process may have had in the first instance, delighting, it would seem, in the sheer play of its means of expression .... This feeling for form as such, freely expanding along predetermined lines and greatly inhibited in certain directions by the lack of controlling types of patterning, should be more clearly understood than it seems to be... these submerged and powerfully controlling impulses to definite form operate as such, regardless of the need for expressing particular concepts or of giving consistent external shape to particular groups of concepts (1921, pp. 60–61).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

References

  • Anderson, J. R. (1983). The Architecture of Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anttila, R. (1972). An Introduction to Historical and Comparative Linguistics. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976). Language and Context: The Acquisition of Pragmatics. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. and MacWhinney, B. (1982). ‘Functionalist approaches to grammar’. In E. Wanner and L. R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language Acquisition: The State of the Art (pp. 173–218). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. and MacWhinney, B. (1987). ‘Competition, variation, and language learning’. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of Language Acquisition (pp. 157–193). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berwick, R. C. and Weinberg, A. S. (1984). The Grammatical Basis of Linguistic Performance. Cambridge , MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bever, T. G. (1986). ‘The aesthetic constraint on cognitive structures’. In M. Brand and R. Harnish (Eds.), The Representation of Knowledge and Belief (pp. 314–356). Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Block, N. (1980). ‘Introduction: What is functionalism?’ In N. Block (Ed.), Readings in the Philosophy of Psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 171–184). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1977). ‘The effect of a pragmatic presupposition on syntactic structure in question answering’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 16, 723–734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1982). ‘Toward a cognitive psychology of syntax: Information processing contributions to sentence formulation’. Psychological Review 89, 1–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1986). ‘Syntactic persistence in language production’. Cognitive Psychology 18, 355–387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1987a). ‘An effect of the accessibility of word forms on sentence structures’. Journal of Memory and Language 26, 119–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1987b). ‘Coordinating words and syntax in speech plans’. In A. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the Psychology of Language (Vol. 3, pp. 337–390). London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. (1987c). ‘Exploring levels of processing in sentence production’. In G. Kempen (Ed.), Natural Language Generation: New Results in Artificial Intelligence, Psychology, and Linguistics (pp. 351–363). Dordrecht: Martinus Nijhoff.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bock, J. K. and Warren, R. K. (1985). ‘Conceptual accessibility and syntactic structure in sentence formulation’. Cognition 21, 47–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolinger, D. (1975). ‘Meaning and form: Some fallacies of asemantic grammar’. In E. F. K. Koerner (Ed.), The Transformational-Generative Paradigm and Modern Linguistic Theory (pp. 3–35). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1973). ‘Structural relationships in children’s utterances: Syntactic or semantic?’ In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive Development and the Acquisition of Language (pp. 197–213). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1985). ‘What shapes children’s grammars?’ In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), The Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition: Vol. 2. Theoretical Issues (pp. 1257–1319). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, D. C, Garrett, M. F., and Zurif, E. B. (1980). ‘Syntactic deficits in Broca’s aphasia’. In D. Caplan (Ed.), Biological Studies of Mental Processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M. D. S. (1976). ‘Children’s first word combinations’. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41 (1, Serial No. 164).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bransford, J. D., Barclay, J. R., and Franks, J. J. (1972). ‘Sentence memory: A constructive versus interpretive approach’. Cognitive Psychology 3, 193–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bresnan, J. and Kaplan, R. M. (1982). ‘Introduction: Grammars as mental representations of language’. In J. Bresnan (Ed.), The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations (pp. xvii–lii). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, R. and McNeill, D. (1966). ‘The “tip of the tongue” phenomenon’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 5, 325–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caramazza, A. and Berndt, R. S. (1978). ‘Semantic and syntactic processes in aphasia: A review of the literature’. Psychological Bulletin 85, 898–918.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1958). ‘Process and content in psycholinguistics’. In Current Trends in the Description and Analysis of Behavior (pp. 175–200). Pittsburgh: University of Pittsburgh Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chien, Y. C. and Lust, B. (1985). ‘The concepts of topic and subject in first language acquisition of Mandarin Chinese’. Child Development 56, 1359–1375.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic Structures. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1959). ‘Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior’. Language 35, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and Mind. New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chomsky, N. (1980). ‘Rules and representations’. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 3, 1–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. (1965). ‘Some structural properties of simple active and passive sentences’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 4, 365–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. and Begun, J. S. (1971). ‘The semantics of sentence subjects’. Language and Speech 14, 34–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • Clark, H. H. and Malt, B. C. (1984). ‘Psychological constraints on language: A commentary on Bresnan and Kaplan and on Givon’. In W. Kintsch, J. R. Miller, and P. G. Poison (Eds.), Method and Tactics in Cognitive Science (pp. 191–214). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, W. E., Soares, C, and Reagan, R. T. (1985). ‘Planning speech: A picture’s word’s worth’. Acta Psychologica 58, 107–114.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. (1986). ‘A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production’. Psychological Review 93, 283–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dell, G. S. and Reich, P. A. (1981). ‘Stages in sentence production: An analysis of speech error data’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 20, 611–629.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • deVilliers, J. G. (1980). ‘The process of rule learning in child speech: A new look’. In K. E. Nelson (Ed.), Children’s Language (pp. 1–44). New York: Gardner Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dik, S. (1978). Functional Grammar. New York: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J. (1986). ‘Consistent and varied training in the theory of automatic and controlled information processing’. Cognition 23, 279–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ertel, S. (1977). ‘Where do the subjects of sentences come from?’ In S. Rosenberg (Ed.), Sentence Production: Developments in Research and Theory (pp. 141–167). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Estival, D. (1985). ‘Syntactic priming of the passive in English’. Text 5, 7–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fay, D. and Cutler, A. (1977). ‘Malapropisms and the structure of the mental lexicon’. Linguistic Inquiry 8, 505–520.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ferreira, F. and Clifton, C, Jr. (1986). ‘The independence of syntactic processing’. Journal of Memory and Language 25, 348–368.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fillenbaum, S. (1966). ‘Memory for gist: Some relevant variables’. Language and Speech 9, 217–227.

    Google Scholar 

  • Firbas, J. (1966). ‘On defining the theme in functional sentence analysis’. Travaux Linguistiques de Prague 1, 267–280.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, L., Taft, L., Roeper, T., Clifton, C, and Ehrlich, K. (1984). ‘Parallel structure: A source of facilitation in sentence comprehension’. Memory and Cognition 12, 421–430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fromkin, V. A. (1971). ‘The non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances’. Language 47, 27–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, E. C. (1979). ‘Discourse without syntax’. In T. Givon (Ed.), Syntax and Semantics: Vol. 12. Discourse and Syntax (pp. 23–49). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, A. (1985). Psycholinguistics: Central Topics. London: Methuen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. F. (1975). ‘The analysis of sentence production’. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 9, pp. 133–177). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. F. (1976). ‘Syntactic processes in sentence production’. In R. J. Wales and E. C. T. Walker (Eds.), New Approaches to Language Mechanisms (pp. 231–256). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. F. (1980). ‘Levels of processing in sentence production’. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language Production (Vol. 1, pp. 177–220). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garrett, M. F. (1982). ‘Production of speech: Observations from normal and pathological language use’. In A. Ellis (Ed.), Normality and Pathology in Cognitive Functions (pp. 19–76). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Giles, H. and Powesland, P. F. (1975). Speech Style and Social Evaluation. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gleitman, L. R. (1984). ‘Biological predispositions to learn language’. In P. Marler and H. S. Terrace (Eds.), The Biology of Learning (pp. 553–584). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Glucksberg, S. (1966). Symbolic Processes. Dubuque, I A: W. C. Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grieve, R. and Wales, R. J. (1973). ‘Passives and topicalization’. British Journal of Psychology 64, 173–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gruber, J. S. (1969). ‘Topicalization in child language’. In D. A. Reibel and S. A. Schane (Ed.), Modern Studies in English: Readings in Transformational Grammar (pp. 422–447). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halliday, M. A. K. (1970). ‘Language structure and language function’. In J. Lyons (Ed.), New Horizons in Linguistics (pp. 140–165). Baltimore: Penguin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M. (1978). ‘Noun animacy and the passive voice: A developmental approach’. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 30, 495–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasher, L. and Zacks, R. T. (1979). ‘Automatic and effortful processes in memory’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 108, 356–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, G. (1963). Thinking: An Introduction to its Experimental Psychology. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • James, C. T., Thompson, J. G., and Baldwin, J. M. (1973). ‘The reconstructive process in sentence memory’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 12, 51–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. and Stevenson, R. (1970). ‘Memory for syntax’. Nature 227, 412.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1979). ‘Micro- and macrodevelopmental changes in language acquisition and other representational systems’. Cognitive Science 3, 91–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, E. L. and Comrie, B. (1977). ‘Noun phrase accessibility and universal grammar’. Linguistic Inquiry 8, 63–99.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, M. H., Bock, J. K., and Keil, F. C. (1986). ‘Prototypicality in a linguistic context: Effects on sentence structure’. Journal of Memory and Language 25, 59–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kroch, A. S. (1982, October). ‘Function and grammar in the history of English periphrastic do’. Paper presented at the 11th Annual Colloquium on New Ways of Analyzing Variation in English, Georgetown University, Washington, D.C.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kroch, A., Myhill, J., and Pintzuk, S. (1982). ‘Understanding do’. In K. Tuite et al. (Eds.), Papers from the 18th Regional Meeting (pp. 282–294). Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuno, S. and Kaburaki, E. (1977). ‘Empathy and syntax’. Linguistic Inquiry 8, 627–672.

    Google Scholar 

  • LaBerge, D. (1981). ‘Automatic information processing: A review’. In J. Long and A. Baddeley (Eds.), Attention and Performance IX (pp. 173–186). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1983). ‘Monitoring and self-repair in speech’. Cognition 14, 41–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levelt, W. J. M. and Kelter, S. (1982). ‘Surface form and memory in question answering’. Cognitive Psychology 14, 78–106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., and Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1967). ‘Perception of the speech code’. Psychological Review 74, 431–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, A. M. and Mattingly, I. G. (1985). ‘The motor theory of speech perception revised’. Cognition 21, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lightfoot, D. (1979). Principles of Diachronic Syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindsley, J. R. (1975). ‘Producing simple utterances: How far ahead do we plan?’ Cognitive Psychology 7, 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1978). ‘Attention in character-classification tasks: Evidence for the automaticity of component stages’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 107, 32–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1979). ‘On the use of a concurrent memory load to measure attention and automaticity’. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5, 189–207.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1980). ‘Attention and automaticity in Stroop and priming tasks: Theory and data’. Cognitive Psychology 12, 523–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1985). ‘Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications, and future directions’. Canadian Journal of Psychology 39, 367–386.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luchins, A. S. (1942). ‘Mechanization in problem-solving: The effect of Einstellung’. Psychological Monographs 54 (6, Whole No. 248).

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (1977). ‘Starting points’. Language 53, 152–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B. (1987). ‘The competition model’. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of Language Acquisition (pp. 249–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, B., Bates, E., and Kliegl, R. (1984). ‘Cue validity and sentence interpretation in English, German, and Italian’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23, 127–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malt, B. C. (1985). ‘The role of discourse structure in understanding anaphora’. Journal of Memory and Language 24, 271–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marcus, M. (1980). A Theory of Syntactic Recognition for Natural Language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matthews, R. J. (1979). ‘Are the grammatical sentences of a language a recursive set?’ Synthese 40, 209–224.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meier, R. P. (1987). ‘Elicited imitation of verb agreement in American Sign Language: Iconically or morphologically determined?’ Journal of Memory and Language 26, 362–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1962). ‘Some psychological studies of grammar’. American Psychologist 17, 748–762.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. (1965). ‘Some preliminaries to psycholinguistics’. American Psychologist 20, 15–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A. and Chomsky, N. (1963). ‘Finitary models of language users’. In R. D. Luce, R. Bush, and E. Galanter (Eds.), Handbook of Mathematical Psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 419–491). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1971). ‘Where do sentences come from?’ In D. D. Steinberg and L. A. Jakobovits (Eds.), Semantics: An Interdisciplinary Reader in Philosophy, Linguistics, and Psychology (pp. 497–529). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. (1980). Lectures on Language Performance. New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osgood, C. E. and Bock, J. K. (1977). ‘Salience and sentencing: Some production principles’. In S. Rosenberg (Ed.). Sentence Production: Developments in Research and Theory (pp. 89–140). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, H. (1886). Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte. Halle: Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. and Garson, B. (1973). ‘Forgetting linguistic information after reading’. Journal of Educational Psychology 65, 135–139.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1984). Language Learnability and Language Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1978). Chronometrie Explorations of Mind. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. and Snyder, C. R. R. (1975). ‘Attention and cognitive control’. In R. L. Solso (Ed.), Information Processing and Cognition (pp. 55–85). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Putnam, W. B. and Street, R. L., Jr. (1984). ‘The conception and perception of noncontent speech performance: Implications for speech-accommodation theory’. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 46, 97–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, A. (1981). Transformational Syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Carlson, M., and Frazier, L. (1983). ‘The interaction of syntax and semantics during sentence processing: Eye movements in the analysis of semantically biased sentences’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22, 358–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rommetveit, R. (1968). Words, Meanings, and Messages. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachs, J. S. (1967). ‘Recognition memory for syntactic and semantic aspects of connected discourse’. Perception and Psychophysics 2, 437–442.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saffran, E. M., Schwartz, M. F., and Marin, O. S. M. (1980). ‘Evidence from aphasia: Isolating the components of a production model’. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language Production (Vol. 1, pp. 221–241). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sapir, E. (1921). Language. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schenkein, J. (1980). ‘A taxonomy for repeating action sequences in natural conversation’. In B. Butterworth (Ed.), Language Production: Vol. 1. Speech and Talk (pp. 21–47). London: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. and Fisk, A. D. (1982). ‘Degree of consistent training: Improvements in search performance and automatic process development’. Perception and Psychophysics 31, 160–168.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). ‘Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention’. Psychological Review 84, 1–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F. (1984). ‘What the classical aphasia categories can’t do for us, and why’. Brain and Language 21, 3–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. F. (1987). ‘Patterns of speech production deficit within and cross aphasia syndromes: Application of a psycholinguistic model’. In M. Coltheart, G. Sartori, and R. Job (Eds.), The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Language (pp. 163–199). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shiffrin, R. M. and Schneider, W. (1977). ‘Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending, and a general theory’. Psychological Review 84, 127–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1966). ‘Grammatical transformations and sentence comprehension in childhood and adulthood’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 5, 219–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slobin, D. I. (1977). ‘Language change in childhood and in history’. In J. Macnamara (Ed.), Language Learning and Thought (pp. 185–214). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stemberger, J. P. (1985). ‘An interactive activation model of language production’. In A. Ellis (Ed.), Progress in the Psychology of Language (Vol. 1, pp. 143–186). London: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanenhaus, M. K., Carlson, G. N., and Seidenberg, M. S. (1985). ‘Do listeners compute linguistic representations?’ In D. Dowty, L. Karttunen, and A. Zwicky (Eds.), Natural Language Parsing: Psychological, Computational, and Theoretical Perspectives (pp. 359–408). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, P. H. and Williams, F. (1968). ‘Generation of active and passive sentences as a function of subject or object focus’. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 7, 246–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wanner, E. (1974). On Remembering, Forgetting, and Understanding Sentences. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weiner, E. J. and Labov, W. (1983). ‘Constraints on the agentless passive’. Journal of Linguistics 19, 29–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., Ironsmith, M., and Goldfein, M. (1974). ‘Selective imitation of the passive construction through modeling’. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 17, 288–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wundt, W. (1900). Die Sprache. Leipzig: Engelmann.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 1989 Kluwer Academic Publishers

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Bock, J.K., Kroch, A.S. (1989). The Isolability of Syntactic Processing. In: Carlson, G.N., Tanenhaus, M.K. (eds) Linguistic Structure in Language Processing. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 7. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-2729-2_5

Download citation

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-2729-2_5

  • Publisher Name: Springer, Dordrecht

  • Print ISBN: 978-1-55608-075-3

  • Online ISBN: 978-94-009-2729-2

  • eBook Packages: Springer Book Archive

Publish with us

Policies and ethics