Advertisement

Thought and Language Intentions and Intensions

  • Mia Gosselin
Part of the Synthese Library book series (SYLI, volume 215)

Abstract

Nominalists and empiricists postulated intentions, which are concepts associated with words. Their theories concerning the way we acquire knowledge and concerning language are much more in accordance with the results of sciences that did not even exist in their days, such as ethology, neurobiology, psycholinguistics, than many philosophical, psychological or linguistic theories that are proposed today. Behaviourism and some cognitivistic theories are, indeed, to a large degree incompatible with the most recent findings in these domains.

Keywords

Sense Organ Synaptic Cleft Linguistic Theory Psychological Phenomenon Mental Phenomenon 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Preview

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Unable to display preview. Download preview PDF.

Notes

  1. 3.
    Leff, William of Ockham, p.123 (quotation from W. of Ockham, Ordinatio, Opera Omnia, ed. Iuntina & Venice, 1574, p.252–253).Google Scholar
  2. 6.
    J. Locke, An Essay Concerning Human Understanding, Everyman’s Library, London, 1961, Vol. II, p.11.Google Scholar
  3. 17.
    D. Hume, A Treatise of Human Nature, Everyman’s Library, Londen, New York, 1964. Vol. 1, p.171.Google Scholar
  4. 18.
    Cf. E. Brhier, Histoire de la Philosophie, Tome II, Fascicule 2, PUF, Paris, 1950, p.406.Google Scholar
  5. 19.
    C. Blakemore, Mechanics of the Mind, Cambridge Univ. Press, Cambridge, 1977, p.16–17.Google Scholar
  6. 27.
    P. Changeux, L’Homme Neuronal, Fayard, Paris, 1983, p.82.Google Scholar
  7. 34.
    J. Piaget, Le langage et les oprations intellectuelles, PUF, Paris, 1963, p.58.Google Scholar
  8. 35.
    J. Piaget, “Le langage et la pense du point de vue gntique”, in: Thinking and Speaking, North Holland Publications, Amsterdam, 1954, p.146.Google Scholar
  9. 36.
    H. Furth, Thought Without Language. Psychological Implications of Deafness, New-York, Collier-Mac Millan, London, 1976, p.12.Google Scholar
  10. 37.
    Cf. P. Olron, Language and Mental Development, Laurence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1977.Google Scholar
  11. 41.
    N. Chomsky, “A Review of B.F. Skinner’s “Verbal Behavior””, in: J.A. Fodor, J.J. Katz, The Structure of Language, Prentice Hall, Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey, 1964, p.547–549.Google Scholar
  12. 49.
    N. Chomsky, Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, MIT Press, Cambridge Massachusetts, 1965, p.206.Google Scholar
  13. 50.
    J.A. Fodor, “The Mind-Body Problem”, Scientific American, 1981, Vol. 244, n. 1, p.114–123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  14. 51.
    J.A. Fodor, The Language of Thought, Harvard Univ. Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts, 1979.Google Scholar
  15. 65.
    G.D. Wassermann, Neurobiological Theory of Psychological Phenomena, Macmillan Press, London and Basingstoke, 1978, p.1.Google Scholar
  16. 67.
    Cf. J.L. Schnopf, D.A. Baylor, “How Photoreceptor Cells Respond to Light”, Scientific American, April 1987, Vol. 256, Nr. 4, p.32–39.Google Scholar
  17. L. Stryer, “The Molecules of Visual Excitation”, Scientific American, July 1987, vol. 257, Nr. 1, p.32–41.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. 74.
    W.R. Uttal, The Psychobiology of Mind, Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, New Jersey, 1978, p.682.Google Scholar
  19. 85.
    F. Basar, H. Rohr, H. Haken, A.J. Mandell, (eds.), Synergetics of the Brains, Springer, Berlin, 1983, p.6.Google Scholar
  20. 99.
    L.A. Cooper, R.N. Shepard, “Turning Something Over in the Mind”, Scientific American, Dec. 1984, Vol. 251, Number 6, p.106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. 109.
    Frank R. Vellutino, “Dyslexia”, Scientific American, March 1987, Vol. 256, Number 3, p.20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990

Authors and Affiliations

  • Mia Gosselin
    • 1
  1. 1.Belgium

Personalised recommendations