Skip to main content

Modelling Mental Activity

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Abstract

Progress in a research programme requires explicit accounts of the nature of what is being researched that are open to critique and, where necessary, revision. As a starting point for shifting away from the ‘taken-for-granted’ approach to discussing mental phenomena that characterises everyday discourse, towards a more analytical approach suitable for a progressive research programme, this chapter explores how we might understand the mind in science education, and the role it plays in relation to common foci of research such as student understanding, knowing and learning. Three complementary levels for thinking about such foci as thinking, knowledge and so forth are considered: the physical level (in terms of physiology and anatomy); the mental level (in terms of how we describe our subjective experience of mental life); and a functional or ‘systems’ level. It is argued that whilst it is important for research in science education to adopt models constrained by what is learned at the physical level (e.g., in neuroscience), the most productive level to model such matters as student knowledge and learning for most research in science education is the system level, analysing the learner’s cognition in terms of a system that processes information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   109.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Hardcover Book
USD   139.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Baars, B. J., & McGovern, K. (1996). Cognitive views of consciousness: What are the facts? How can we explain them? In M. Velmans (Ed.), The science of consciousness: Psychological, neuropsychological and clinical reviews (pp. 63–95). London: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Behar, R. (2001). Yellow marigolds for Ochun: An experiment in feminist ethnographic fiction. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 14(2), 107–116. doi:10.1080/09518390010023630.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruner, J. S. (1987). The transactional self. In J. Bruner & H. Haste (Eds.), Making sense: The child’s construction of the world (pp. 81–96). London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Changeux, J.-P. (1983/1997). Neuronal man: The biology of mind (L. Garey, Trans.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Claxton, G. (2005). The wayward mind: An intimate history of the unconscious. London: Little Brown.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crick, F., & Koch, C. (1990). Towards a neurobiological theory of consciousness. Seminars in the Neurosciences, 2, 263–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, J. K., & Treagust, D. F. (Eds.). (2009). Multiple representations in chemical education. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnstone, A. H. (2000). Teaching of chemistry – Logical or psychological? Chemistry Education: Research and Practice in Europe, 1(1), 9–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980a). Conceptual metaphor in everyday language. The Journal of Philosophy, 77(8), 453–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Libet, B. (1996). Neural processes in the production of conscious experience. In M. Velmans (Ed.), The science of consciousness: Psychological, neuropsychological and clinical reviews (pp. 96–117). London: Routledge.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Mahowald, M. W., & Schenck, C. H. (2000). Parasomnias: Sleepwalking and the law. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 4(4), 321–339. doi:10.1053/smrv.1999.0078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Norum, K. E. (2000). School patterns: A sextet. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 13(3), 239–250. doi:10.1080/09518390050019659.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rees, G. (2007). Neural correlates of the contents of visual awareness in humans. In J. Driver, P. Haggard, & T. Shallice (Eds.), Mental processes in the human brain (pp. 187–202). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taber, K. S. (2009a). Learning at the symbolic level. In J. K. Gilbert & D. F. Treagust (Eds.), Multiple representations in chemical education (pp. 75–108). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, K. S. (2012). Recognising quality in reports of chemistry education research and practice. Chemistry Education Research and Practice, 13(1), 4–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taber, K. S. (2013c). Revisiting the chemistry triplet: Drawing upon the nature of chemical knowledge and the psychology of learning to inform chemistry education. Chemistry Education Research and Practice, 14(2), 156–168. doi:10.1039/C3RP00012E.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trainor, A. (2003). Poem. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 16(5), 727–727. doi:10.1080/0951839032000143039.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1934/1986). Thought and language. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Taber, K.S. (2013). Modelling Mental Activity. In: Modelling Learners and Learning in Science Education. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7648-7_3

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics