Skip to main content

Rapid Automatized Naming and Its Unique Contribution to Reading: Evidence from Chinese Dyslexia

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 8))

Abstract

Rapid automatized naming (RAN) is suggested to be a significant predictor of reading. However, how it is related to reading and whether it contributes uniquely to reading with phonological awareness statistically controlled is unclear. In this chapter, we reported a study of 45 fourth and fifth grade Chinese children with dyslexia and 45 age-matched controls. They were administrated processing speed tasks, auditory temporal processing, RAN, phonological awareness, Chinese character recognition, and timed word list reading. Results showed that Chinese dyslexic children performed poorer than typical developing children in all tasks. And principal component analyses revealed that RAN loaded in both phonological processing and processing speed component. RAN uniquely predicted Chinese timed and untimed word reading, while phonological awareness predicted only untimed Chinese word reading. The underlying mechanism and the role of RAN in Chinese were discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

References

  • Au, A., & Lovegrove, B. (2001). Temporal processing ability in above average and average readers. Perception & Psychophysics, 63, 148–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Journal of Educational Psychology, 5, 69–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H. W., Gillispie, M., Leonard, L. B., Kail, R. V., & Miller, C. A. (2002). The role of speed of processing, rapid naming, and phonological awareness in reading achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 509–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Stringer, R., Siegel, L., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Why the timing deficit hypothesis does not explain reading disability in adults. Reading and Writing, 15, 73–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chow, B. W. Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Burgess, S. (2005). Phonological processing skills and early reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese kindergarteners learning to read English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 81–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Denckla, M. B. (2001). The relationship of rapid serial naming and word naming in normally developing readers: An exploratory model. Reading and Writing, 14, 673–705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (RAN): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14, 471–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ding, D., Liu, X., Li, L., Zhao, H., Yao, J., & Tian, H. (2002). A study on features of being literate in reading disabled children. Psychological Development and Education, 18, 64–67 (Original work published in Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theories, findings and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Liao, C. H. (2008). Rapid naming speed and reading across languages that vary in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing, 21, 885–903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Protopapas, A., Papadopoulos, T. C., Skaloumbakas, C., & Parrila, R. (2010). Auditory temporal processing and dyslexia in an orthographically consistent language. Cortex, 46, 1330–1344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., Chan, D. W. O., Tsang, S. M., & Lee, S. H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple-deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38, 543–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S. H., & Lai, D. N. C. (1999). Naming-speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Journal of Individual Differences, 11, 173–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in reading achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 401–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Goetz, K., Gooch, D., Adams, J., & Snowling, M. J. (2007). Paired-associate learning, phoneme awareness, and learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 150–166.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ingram, T. T. (1963). Delayed development of speech with special reference to dyslexia. Proceedings of the Royal Society of Medicine, 56, 199–203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ju, D., & Jackson, N. E. (1995). Graphic and phonological processing in Chinese character identification. Journal of Reading Behavior, 27, 299–313.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kail, R. V., & Hall, L. K. (1994). Processing speed, naming speed, and reading. Developmental Psychology, 30, 949–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., & Pfeiffer, S. L. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lei, L., Pan, J., Liu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Li, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2011). Developmental trajectories of reading development and impairment from ages 3 to 8 years in Chinese children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 212–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2009). Rapid automatized naming (RAN) taps a mechanism that places constraints on the developmental of early reading fluency. Psychological Science, 20, 1040–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, H., Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Liu, H. Y., & Xue, J. (2009). Paired associate learning in Chinese children with dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 135–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liao, C. H., Georgiou, G. K., & Parrila, R. (2008). Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition. Reading and Writing, 21(3), 231–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing, 15, 653–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meng, X., Sai, X., Wang, C., Wang, J., Sha, S., & Zhou, X. (2005). Auditory and speech processing and reading development in Chinese school children: behavioural and ERP evidence. Dyslexia, 11, 292–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Modern Chinese Frequency Dictionary. (1986). [in Chinese]. Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moll, K., Fussenegger, B., Willburger, E., & Landerl, K. (2009). RAN is not a measure of orthographic processing. Evidence from the asymmetric German orthography. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 1–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pan, J., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., & Li, H. (2011). What’s in the naming? A 5-year longitudinal study of early rapid naming and phonological sensitivity in relation to subsequent reading skills in both native Chinese and English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 103, 897–908.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, T. K., Snowling, M. J., & de Jong, P. F. (2004). A cross-linguistic comparison of children learning to read in English and Dutch. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 785–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112, 43–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Garwood, H., & Quinlan, P. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 46–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1996). Standard progressive matrices. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-Prime reference guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Meng, X., & Lai, A. (2003). The lexical representation and processing in Chinese-speaking poor readers. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 191–214). Westport, CT: Praeger Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., & Wu, N. (2006). Growth of orthography-phonology knowledge in Chinese writing system. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates, & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), Handbook of east Asian psycholinguisitics: Chinese (pp. 103–113). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P. (1980). Language and reading: Some perceptual prerequisites. Annals of Dyslexia, 30, 170–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., & Gaab, N. (2006). Dynamic auditory processing, musical experience, and language development. Trends in Neurosicences, 29, 382–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: Rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 83–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vaessen, A., Gerretsen, P., & Blomert, L. (2009). Naming problems do not reflect a second independent core deficit in dyslexia: Double deficits explored. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103, 202–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, X. L., & Tao, B. P. (1993). Chinese character recognition test battery and assessment scale for primary school children. Shanghai, China: Shanghai Education Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, S., Milne, E., Rosen, S., Hansen, P., Swettenham, J., Frith, U., et al. (2006). The role of sensorimotor impairments in dyslexia: A multiple case study of dyslexic children. Developmental Science, 9, 237–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Mayringer, H. (2002). Dysfluent reading in the absence of spelling difficulties: A specific disability in regular orthographies. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94, 272–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Xue, J. (2008). Cross-sectional study on reading mechanism of Chinese developmental dyslexia and subtypes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Beijing Normal University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, H., & Wang, X. (1985). 瑞文标准推理测验手册 [Raven’s IQ Reasoning Standardized Test]. Beijing: Department of Psychology, Beijing Normal University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J., Bertrand, D., Tórth, D., Csépe, V., Reis, A., Faísca, L., et al. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21, 551–559.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Hua Shu .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Pan, J., Shu, H. (2014). Rapid Automatized Naming and Its Unique Contribution to Reading: Evidence from Chinese Dyslexia. In: Chen, X., Wang, Q., Luo, Y. (eds) Reading Development and Difficulties in Monolingual and Bilingual Chinese Children. Literacy Studies, vol 8. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-7380-6_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics