Skip to main content

Part of the book series: Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics ((SITP,volume 42))

Abstract

In the same way that listeners must take into account a hypothetical speaker to achieve successful communication, it is argued that speakers must take into account their hypothetical listeners to get their message across. Because child speakers are not yet able to accommodate their listener’s communicative needs, they often prefer the use of a potentially ambiguous pronoun to a more explicit full noun phrase. Mature speakers, on the other hand, are usually able to avoid misunderstanding. They tend to block the use of pronouns when using a pronoun will result in a non-intended interpretation. Also, they only deviate from canonical word order when the listener has other cues than word order to arrive at the intended meaning of the sentence. This pattern of production of pronouns and word order in discourse is accounted for by the same Optimality Theoretic grammar that was shown in previous chapters to account for the observed pattern of comprehension of pronouns and word order at the sentence level.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

eBook
USD 16.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Taken from the website http://www.apple.com/au/iphone/features/siri.html on March 11, 2012.

  2. 2.

    The Dutch narratives in this chapter can be found in the Asymmetries corpus in the narrative section of CHILDES (MacWhinney and Snow 1985, 1990).

References

  • Aissen, Judith. 2003. Differential object marking: Iconicity vs. economy. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 21: 435–483.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, John R., Daniel Bothell, Michael D. Byrne, Scott Douglass, Christian Lebiere, and Yulin Qin. 2004. An integrated theory of the mind. Psychological Review 111: 1036–1060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anttila, Arto. 1997. Deriving variation from grammar. In Variation, change and phonological theory, ed. Frans Hinskens, Roeland van Hout, and Leo Wetzels, 35–68. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, Mira. 1988. Referring and accessibility. Journal of Linguistics 24: 65–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ariel, Mira. 1990. Accessing noun-phrase antecedents. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, Jennifer E. 1998. Reference form and discourse patterns. Dissertation, Stanford University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arnold, Jennifer E. 2008. Reference production: Production-internal and addressee-oriented processes. Language & Cognitive Processes 23: 495–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berman, Stephen R. 1991. On the semantics and logical form of WH-clauses. Dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, Gerlof. 2008. Starting a sentence in Dutch: A corpus study of subject- and object fronting. Dissertation, University of Groningen. Groningen Dissertations in Linguistics 66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bouma, Gerlof, and Petra Hendriks. 2012. Partial word order freezing in Dutch. Journal of Logic, Language and Information 21: 53–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burzio, Luigi. 1998. Anaphora and soft constraints. In Is the best good enough? Optimality and competition in syntax, ed. Pilar Barbosa, Danny Fox, Paul Hagstrom, Martha McGinnis, and David Pesetsky, 93–113. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, Aimee L., Patricia Brooks, and Michael Tomasello. 2000. Factors affecting young children’s use of pronouns as referring expressions. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 43: 1337–1349.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cannizzaro, Gisi. 2012. Early word order and animacy. Dissertation, University of Groningen.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chafe, Wallace L. 1976. Givenness, contrastiveness, definiteness, subjects, topics, and point of view. In Subject and topic, ed. Charles Li, 25–56. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comrie, Bernard. 1979. Definite and animate direct objects: A natural class. Linguistica Silesiana 3: 13–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cat, Cécile. 2011. Information tracking and encoding in early L1: Linguistic competence vs. cognitive limitations. Journal of Child Language 38: 828–860.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Hoop, Helen, and Irene Krämer. 2005/6. Children’s optimal interpretations of indefinite subjects and objects. Language Acquisition 13: 103–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flack, Kathryn. 2007. Ambiguity avoidance as contrast preservation: Case and word order freezing in Japanese. In Umass occasional papers in linguistics 32: Papers in optimality theory III, ed. Leah Bateman, Michael O’Keefe, Ehren Reilly, and Adam Werle, 57–89. Booksurge Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frazier, Lyn. 1985. Syntactic complexity. In Natural language parsing, ed. David Dowty, Lauri Karttunen, and Arnold Zwicky, 129–189. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gernsbacher, Morton Ann, and David J. Hargreaves. 1988. Accessing sentence participants: The advantage of first mention. Journal of Memory and Language 27: 699–717.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Givón, Talmy. 1983. Topic and continuity in discourse: An introduction. In Topic continuity in discourse: A quantitative cross-language study, ed. Talmy Givón, 1–42. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grice, H. Paul. 1975. Logic and conversation. In Speech acts, ed. Peter Cole and Jerry L. Morgan, 41–58. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grosz, Barbara J., Aravind K. Joshi, and Scott Weinstein. 1995. Centering: A framework for modelling the local coherence of discourse. Computational Linguistics 21: 203–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gundel, Jeanette K., Nancy Hedberg, and Ron Zacharski. 1993. Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language 69: 274–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks, Petra, and Helen de Hoop. 2001. Optimality theoretic semantics. Linguistics and Philosophy 24: 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks, Petra, and Jennifer Spenader. 2005/6. When production precedes comprehension: An optimization approach to the acquisition of pronouns. Language Acquisition 13: 319–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks, Petra, Christina Englert, Ellis Wubs, and John Hoeks. 2008. Age differences in adults’ use of referring expressions. Journal of Logic, Language and Information 17: 443–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks, Petra, Helen de Hoop, Irene Krämer, Henriëtte de Swart, and Joost Zwarts. 2010. Conflicts in interpretation. London: Equinox Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hendriks, Petra, Charlotte Koster, and John Hoeks. in press. Referential choice across the lifespan: Why children and elderly adults produce ambiguous pronouns. Language and Cognitive Processes. Published online: 03 Apr 2013. DOI: 10.1080/01690965.2013.766356.

  • Hickmann, Maya, and Henriette Hendriks. 1999. Cohesion and anaphora in children’s narratives: A comparison of English, French, German and Mandarin Chinese. Journal of Child Language 26: 291–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaan, Edith. 1999. Sensitivity to NP-type: Processing subject-object ambiguities in Dutch. Journal of Semantics 15: 335–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaan, Edith. 2001. Subject-object order ambiguities and the nature of the second NP. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 30: 527–545.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaiser, Elsi, and John C. Trueswell. 2011. Investigating the interpretation of pronouns and demonstratives in Finnish: Going beyond salience. In The processing and acquisition of reference, ed. Edward Gibson and Neil J. Pearlmutter, 323–353. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, Annette. 1985. Language and cognitive processes from a developmental perspective. Language & Cognitive Processes 1: 61–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karttunen, Lauri. 1977. Syntax and semantics of questions. Linguistics and Philosophy 1: 1–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, Hanjung. 2001. Markedness and word order freezing. In Formal and empirical issues in optimality theoretic syntax, ed. Peter Sells, 63–127. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, Brian, and Catherine E. Snow. 1985. The child language data exchange system. Journal of Child Language 12: 271–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacWhinney, Brian, and Catherine E. Snow. 1990. The child language data exchange system: An update. Journal of Child Language 17: 457–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manning, Chris D., and Hinrich Schütze. 1999. Foundations of statistical natural language processing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prince, Ellen. 1992. The ZPG letter: Subjects, definiteness, and information-status. In Discourse description: Diverse linguistic analyses of a fund-raising text, ed. William C. Mann and Sandra A. Thompson, 295–326. Amsterdam: Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, Tanya. 2004. The processing cost of reference set computation: Acquisition of stress shift and focus. Language Acquisition 12: 109–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reinhart, Tanya. 2006. Interface strategies. Optimal and costly computations. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ruigendijk, Esther, Naama Friedmann, Rama Novogrodsky, and Noga Balaban. 2010. Symmetry in comprehension and production of pronouns: A comparison of German and Hebrew. Lingua 120: 1991–2005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, Hyun-joo, and Cynthia Fisher. 2005. Who’s “she”? Discourse prominence influences preschoolers’ comprehension of pronouns. Journal of Memory and Language 52: 29–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Song, Hyun-joo, and Cynthia Fisher. 2007. Discourse prominence effects on 2.5-year-old children’s interpretation of pronouns. Lingua 117: 1959–1987.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spenader, Jennifer, Erik-Jan Smits, and Petra Hendriks. 2009. Coherent discourse solves the pronoun interpretation problem. Journal of Child Language 36: 23–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Hout, Angeliek, Kaitlyn Harrigan, and Jill de Villiers. 2010. Asymmetries in the acquisition of definite and indefinite noun phrases. Lingua 120: 1973–1990.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Rij, Jacolien, Hedderik van Rijn, and Petra Hendriks. 2010. Cognitive architectures and language acquisition: A case study in pronoun comprehension. Journal of Child Language 37: 731–766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Rij, Jacolien, Hedderik van Rijn, and Petra Hendriks. in press. How WM load influences linguistic processing in adults: A computational model of pronoun interpretation in discourse. TopiCS. Published online: 11 June 2013. DOI: 10.1111/tops.12029.

  • Wasow, Thomas, Amy Perfors, and David Beaver. 2005. The puzzle of ambiguity. In Morphology and the web of grammar: Essays in memory of Steven G. Lapointe, ed. C. Orhan and Peter Sells. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittek, Angelika, and Michael Tomasello. 2005. Young children’s sensitivity to listener knowledge and perceptual context in choosing referring expressions. Applied PsychoLinguistics 26: 541–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wubs, Ellis, Petra Hendriks, John Hoeks, and Charlotte Koster. 2009. Tell me a story! Children’s capacity for topic shift. In Proceedings of the 3rd conference on generative approaches to language acquisition North America (GALANA 2008), ed. Jean Crawford, Koichi Otaki, and Masahiko Takahashi, 313–324. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2014 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Hendriks, P. (2014). The Speaker’s Perspective. In: Asymmetries between Language Production and Comprehension. Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, vol 42. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-6901-4_4

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics