Advertisement

Great Expectations in Phonology? Second Language Acquisition Research and Its Relation to the Teaching of Older and Younger Learners

  • Martha Young-ScholtenEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Educational Linguistics book series (EDUL, volume 16)

Abstract

In 1985, Patsy Lightbown presented a set of generalizations based on research on the second language acquisition of morphosyntax regarding what teachers should be aware of. These “great expectations” give teachers the confidence to move away from an externally imposed grammar-translation-based syllabus to a learner-driven internal syllabus. In phonology, research since the 1970s points to the need to encourage teachers to develop great expectations for their learners’ acquisition of a new sound system. This is particularly true for young, prepuberty learners, but unlike in the acquisition of morphosyntax, these great expectations require teachers to think more seriously about the input their learners receive.

Keywords

Great Expectation Syllable Structure Constraint Ranking Segmental Phonology Orthographic Input 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

References

  1. Abrahamsson, N. 2003. Development and recoverability of L2 codas: A longitudinal study of Chinese-Swedish interphonology. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 25: 313–349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Akita, M. 1998. A longitudinal study of Japanese EFL learners’ interlanguage phonology. Paper presented at EUROSLA 8. Paris.Google Scholar
  3. Archibald, J. 1992. Transfer of L2 parameter settings: Some empirical evidence from Polish metrics. Canadian Journal of Linguistics 37: 301–339.Google Scholar
  4. Bassetti, B. 2009. Orthographic input and second language phonology. In Input matters in SLA, ed. T. Piske and M. Young-Scholten, 176–190. Bristol/Buffalo/Toronto: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
  5. Best, C.T. 1995. A direct realist view of cross-language speech perception. In Speech perception and linguistic experience, ed. W. Strange, 171–204. Timonium: York Press.Google Scholar
  6. Bongaerts, T. 1999. Ultimate attainment in L2 pronunciation: The case of very advanced late L2 learners. In Second language acquisition and the critical period hypothesis, ed. D. Birdsong, 133–159. London: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
  7. Broselow, E. 1987. Non-obvious transfer: On predicting epenthesis errors. In Interlanguage phonology, ed. G. Ioup and S. Weinberger. Rowley: Newbury House.Google Scholar
  8. Broselow, E., and D. Finer. 1991. Parameter setting and transfer in second language phonology and syntax. Second Language Research 7: 35–59.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  9. Broselow, E., and H.-B. Park. 1995. Mora conservation in second language prosody. In Phonological acquisition and phonological theory, ed. J. Archibald, 151–168. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
  10. Brown, C., and J. Matthews. 1997. The role of feature geometry in the development of phonemic contrasts. In Focus on phonological acquisition, ed. S.J. Hannahs and M. Young-Scholten, 67–113. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  11. Carlisle, R. 1998. The acquisition of onsets in a markedness relationship: A longitudinal study. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 30: 245–260.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chomsky, N., and M. Halle. 1968. The sound pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
  13. Cutler, A. 1984. Stress and accent in language production and understanding. In Intonation, accent and rhythm: Studies in discourse phonology, ed. D. Gibbon and H. Richter, 77–90. Berlin/New York: de Gruyter.Google Scholar
  14. Eckman, F. 1977. Markedness and the contrastive analysis hypothesis. Language Learning 27: 315–330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Eckman, F. 1981. On the naturalness of interlanguage phonological rules. Language Learning 31: 195–216.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  16. Eckman, F. 2008. Typological markedness and second language phonology. In Phonology and second language acquisition, ed. J.G.H. Edwards and M.L. Zampini. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  17. Eckman, F., and G. Iverson. 1997. Structure preservation in interlanguage phonology. In Focus on phonological acquisition, ed. S.J. Hannahs and M. Young-Scholten, 183–208. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  18. Edwards, J.G.H. 2006. Acquiring a non-native phonology: Linguistic constraints and social barriers. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
  19. Escudero, P., and P. Boersma. 2004. Bridging the gap between L2 speech perception research and phonological theory. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 26: 551–585.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Fikkert, P. 1994. On the acquisition of prosodic structure. Amsterdam: Holland Institute of Generative Linguistics.Google Scholar
  21. Flege, J.E. 1995. Second language speech learning. Findings and problems. In Speech perception and linguistic experience, ed. W. Strange, 233–273. Timonium: York Press.Google Scholar
  22. Gut, U. 2009. Non-native speech. A corpus-based analysis of phonological and phonetic properties of L2 English and German. Frankfurt am Main: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
  23. Hancin-Bhatt, B. 2008. Second language phonology in optimality theory. In Phonology and second language acquisition, ed. H.J. Edwards and M.L. Zampini, 117–146. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  24. Hayes-Harb, R., and K. Masuda. 2008. Development of the ability to lexically encode novel L2 phonemic contrasts. Second Language Research 24: 5–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Haznedar, B., and B.D. Schwartz. 1997. Are there optional infinitives in child L2 acquisition? In Proceedings of the 21st annual Boston University conference on language development, ed. E. Hughes, M. Hughes, and A. Greenhill, 257–268. Somerville: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
  26. Herschensohn, J. 2007. Language development and age. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Ioup, G., and S. Weinberger. 1987. Interlanguage phonology. Rowley: Newbury House.Google Scholar
  28. Ioup, G., E. Boustagui, M. El Tigi, and M. Moselle. 1994. Reexamining the critical period hypothesis: A case study of successful adult SLA in a naturalistic environment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 16: 73–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Jenkins, J. 2000. The phonology of English as an international language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Jenkins, J. 2002. A sociolinguistic based, empirically researched pronunciation syllabus for English as an international language. Applied Linguistics 23: 83–103.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Jusczyk, P. 1997. The discovery of spoken language. London: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  32. Krashen, S. 1985. The input hypothesis. Issues and implications. London: Longman.Google Scholar
  33. Kuhl, P., and P. Iverson. 1995. Linguistic experience and the “perceptual magnet effect”. In Speech perception and linguistic experience: Issues in cross-language research, ed. W. Strange, 121–154. Timonium: York Press.Google Scholar
  34. Lado, R. 1957. Linguistics across cultures. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
  35. Lenneberg, E. 1967. The biological foundations of language. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
  36. Leopold, W. 1944. Speech development of a bilingual child. A linguist’s record. Sound learning in the first two years. Evanston: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
  37. Leung, A.H.-C. 2011. [fɪlɪpɪno] or [pɪlɪpɪno]? Children’s L2 English acquisition under the influence of multiple varieties. JSLS Conference Handbook 89–92.Google Scholar
  38. Leung, A.H.-C. 2012. Bad influence? An investigation into the purported negative influence of foreign domestic helpers on children’s second language English acquisition. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development 33(2): 133–148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  39. Lightbown, P. 1985. Great expectations. Second-language acquisition research and classroom teaching. Applied Linguistics 6(2): 173–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Long, M. 1990. Maturational constraints on language development. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 12: 251–285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Major, R. 2008. Transfer in second language phonology: A review. In Phonology and second language acquisition, ed. H.J. Edwards and M.L. Zampini, 63–94. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  42. Moyer, A. 2009. Input as a critical means to an end. Quantity and quality of experience in L2 phonological attainment. In Input matters in second language acquisition, ed. T. Piske and M. Young-Scholten, 159–174. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
  43. Patkowski, M.S. 1990. Age and accent in a second language: A reply to James Emil Flege. Applied Linguistics 11: 73–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Prévost, P., and L. White. 2000. Missing surface inflection or impairment in second language acquisition? Evidence from tense and agreement. Second Language Research 16: 103–134.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  45. Preyer, W. 1889. The mind of the child. New York: Appleton.Google Scholar
  46. Prince, A., and P. Smolenksy. 2004. Optimality theory: Constraint interaction in generative grammar. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
  47. Rafat, Y. 2011. Orthography-induced transfer in the production of adult novice English speaking learners of Spanish. PhD dissertation, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
  48. Riney, T. 1990. Age and open syllable production in interlanguage phonology. In Proceedings of the 10th meeting of the Second Language Research Forum. Volume 2: Variability in second language acquisition, ed. H. Burmeister and P. Rounds. Eugene: Department of Linguistics, University of Oregon.Google Scholar
  49. Rogerson-Revell, P. 2011. English phonology and pronunciation teaching. London: Continuum.Google Scholar
  50. Rose, Y. 2000. Headedness and prosodic licensing in the first language acquisition of phonology. PhD dissertation, McGill University.Google Scholar
  51. Sabourin, L., C. Brien, and M.-C. Tremblay. 2013. Electrophysiology of second language processing: The past, present and future. In Multiple perspectives on second language acquisition, ed. M.d P. García Mayo, M.J. Gutierrez-Mangado, and M. Martínez Adrián, 221–242. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  52. Sato, C.J. 1984. Phonological processes in second language acquisition: Another look at interlanguage syllable structure. Language Learning 34: 43–57.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Scovel, T. 1981. The recognition of foreign accents in English and its implications for psycholinguistic theories of language acquisition. In Proceedings of the 5th International Association of Applied Linguistics, ed. J.-G. Savard and L. Laforge, 389–401. Montreal: Laval University Press.Google Scholar
  54. Selkirk, E. 1984. Phonology and syntax. The relation between sound and structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  55. Setter, J., and J. Jenkins. 2005. Pronunciation. State-of-the-art review article. Language Teaching 38: 1–17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  56. Smith, N.V. 1973. The acquisition of phonology: A case study. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  57. Snow, C.E., and M. Hoefnagel-Höhle. 1982. Age differences in the pronunciation of foreign sounds. In Child-adult differences in second language acquisition, ed. S. Krashen, M. Long, and R. Scarcella. Rowley: Newbury House.Google Scholar
  58. Sumdangdej, S. 2007. Input and the acquisition of suprasegmental phonology in English by Thai school children. PhD dissertation, Durham University.Google Scholar
  59. Tropf, H. 1986. Sonority as a variability factor in second language phonology. In Sound patterns in second language acquisition, ed. A. James and J. Leather, 173–192. Dordrecht: Foris.Google Scholar
  60. Weinberger, S. 1988. Theoretical foundations of second language phonology. PhD dissertation, University of Washington.Google Scholar
  61. Winitz, H., B. Gillespie, and J. Starcev. 1995. The development of English speech patterns of a 7-year-old Polish-speaking child. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 24: 117–143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  62. Wode, H. 1993. The development of phonological abilities. In Progression and regression in language, ed. K. Hyltenstam and A. Viberg, 415–438. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
  63. Wode, H. 2009. Developing non-native pronunciation in immersion settings. In Input matters in second language acquisition, ed. T. Piske and M. Young-Scholten, 238–256. Bristol: Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
  64. Young-Scholten, M. 1995. The negative effects of ‘positive’ evidence on L2 phonology. In The current state of interlanguage, ed. L. Eubank, L. Selinker, and M. Sharwood Smith, 107–121. Amsterdam: Benjamins.Google Scholar
  65. Young-Scholten, M. 2004. Prosodic constraints on allophonic distribution in adult L2 acquisition. International Journal of Bilingualism 8: 67–77.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Young-Scholten, M. 2011. Development in phonology. Another perspective on age. In Achievements and perspectives in SLA of speech, ed. K. Dziubalska-Kołaczyk, M. Wrembel, and M. Kul, 331–342. Berlin: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
  67. Young-Scholten, M., and J. Archibald. 2000. Second language syllable structure. In Second language acquisition and linguistic theory, ed. J. Archibald, 64–101. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2013

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.School of English Literature, Language and LinguisticsNewcastle UniversityNewcastle upon TyneUK

Personalised recommendations