Advertisement

Seeing as a Non-Experiental Mental State: The Case from Synesthesia and Visual Imagery

  • Berit BrogaardEmail author
Chapter
Part of the Studies in Brain and Mind book series (SIBM, volume 6)

Abstract

The paper argues that the English verb ‘to see’ can denote three different kinds of conscious states of seeing, involving visual experiences, visual seeming states and introspective seeming states, respectively. The case for the claim that there are three kinds of seeing comes from synesthesia and visual imagery. Synesthesia is a relatively rare neurological condition in which stimulation in one sensory or cognitive stream involuntarily leads to associated experiences in a second unstimulated stream. Visual synesthesia is often considered a case of illusory visual experience. This, however, turns out to be a questionable characterization, as there is evidence suggesting that the brain must cognitively process the stimulus in order for the associated synesthetic experience to arise. Furthermore, some very vivid, visual forms of synesthesia do not involve additional processing in the visual cortex. Visual synesthetic experience is likely to be a non-veridical state of seeming rather than an illusory visual experience. Visual seeming states are cognitive states distinct from visual experiences in terms of their representational richness and their neural correlates. Visual seeming states that are non-deviantly causally related to the states of affair they represent constitute a type of non-experiental seeing. Introspective seeming states that are non-deviantly causally related to underlying visual images constitute a second type of non-experiental seeing. The English verb ‘to see’ can denote all three types of seeing, which is to say that ‘to see’ is polysemous.

Keywords

Visual Experience Belief State Visual Imagery Gang Member Visual Cortical Area 
These keywords were added by machine and not by the authors. This process is experimental and the keywords may be updated as the learning algorithm improves.

Notes

Acknowledgements

I am grateful to David Bourget, Alex Byrne, David J. Chalmers, Ophelia Deroy, David Eagleman, Kristian Marlow, Sydney Shoemaker and Juha Silvanto for discussion of these and related issues. Special thanks to Ophelia Deroy for written comments on the paper.

References

  1. Aleman, A.A., G.-J.M. Rutten, M.M. Sitskoorn, G. Dautzenberg, and N.F. Ramsey. 2001. Activation of striate cortex in the absence of visual stimulation: An fMRI study of synesthesia. Neuroreport 12: 2827–2830.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  2. Baron-Cohen, S., M. Wyke, and C. Binnie. 1987. Hearing words and seeing colors: An experimental investigation of synesthesia. Perception 16: 761–767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  3. Beck, J. 1966. Effect of orientation and of shape similarity on perceptual grouping. Perception & Psychophysics 1: 300–302.Google Scholar
  4. Bor, D., J. Billington, and S. Baron-Cohen. 2007. Savant memory for digits in a case of synaesthesia and Asperger syndrome is related to hyperactivity in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Neurocase 13: 311–319.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  5. Bourget, D. 2010. Intensional and phenomenal uses of perceptual verbs, Chapter in ANU Dissertation.Google Scholar
  6. Brogaard, B. 2010. Do we perceive natural kind properties?, Philosophical Studies 162(1) (2013): 35–42.Google Scholar
  7. Brogaard, B. 2011. Primitive knowledge disjunctivism. Philosophical Issues 21: 45–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  8. Brogaard, B. 2012. Perceptual reports. In Oxford handbook for the philosophy of perception, ed. M. Matthen. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  9. Brogaard, B. 2013. Phenomenal seemings and sensible dogmatism, forthcoming. In Seemings and justification: New essays on dogmatism and phenomenal conservatism, ed. C. Tucker. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  10. Brogaard, B., S. Vanni, and J. Silvanto. 2012. Seeing mathematics: perceptual experience and brain activity in acquired synesthesia. Neurocase (in press)Google Scholar
  11. Chalmers, D.J., R.M. French, and D.R. Hofstadter. 1992. High-level perception, representation, and analogy: A critique of artificial intelligence methodology. Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 4: 185–211.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Chisholm, R.M. 1957. Perceiving: A philosophical study. Ithaca: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
  13. Church, J. 2000. ‘Seeing As’ and the double bind of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies 7: 99–111.Google Scholar
  14. Cytowic, R.E. 1989. Synesthesia: A union of the senses. New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  15. Cytowic, R.E., and D.M. Eagleman. 2009. Wednesday is indigo blue. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
  16. Danker, J.F., and J.R. Anderson. 2010. The ghosts of brain states past: Remembering reactivates the brain regions engaged during encoding. Psychological Bulletin 136: 87–102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  17. Dehaene, S., N. Molko, L. Cohen, and A. Wilson. 2004. Arithmetic and the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 14: 218–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  18. Delk, J.L., and S. Fillenbaum. 1965. Differences in perceived colour as a function of characteristic color. The American Journal of Psychology 78(2): 290–293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  19. Denys, K., W. Vanduffel, D. Fize, K. Nelissen, H. Peuskens, D. Van Essen, and G.A. Orban. 2004. The processing of visual shape in the cerebral cortex of human and nonhuman primate: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Journal of Neuroscience 24: 2551–2565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Dixon, M.J., D. Smilek, and P.M. Merikle. 2004. Not all synaesthetes are created equal: Projector versus associator synaesthetes. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience 4: 335–343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Eagleman, D.M., A.D. Kagan, S.S. Nelson, D. Sagaram, and A.K. Sarma. 2007. A standardized test battery for the study of synesthesia. Journal of Neuroscience Methods 159: 139–145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Edquist, J., A.N. Rich, C. Brinkman, and J.B. Mattingly. 2006. Do synaesthetic colours act as unique features in a visual search? Cortex 42: 222–231.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  23. Eichenbaum, H. 2004. Hippocampus: Cognitive processes and neural representations that underlie declarative memory. Neuron 44: 109–120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  24. Higginbotham, J.T. 1983. The logic of perceptual reports: An extensional alternative to situation semantics. Journal of Philosophy 80: 100–127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Kahn, I., L. Davachi, and A.D. Wagner. 2004. Functional-neuroanatomic correlates of recollection: Implications for models of recognition memory. Journal of Neuroscience 24: 4172–4180.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  26. Kosslyn, S.M. 2005. Mental images and the brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology 22: 333–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Kvart, I. 1993. Seeing that and seeing as. Noûs 27: 279–302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  28. Laeng, B., F. Svartdal, and H. Oelmann. 2004. Does color synesthesia pose a paradox for early-selection theories of attention? Psychological Science 15: 277–281.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  29. Lewis, D. 1988. Veridical hallucinations and prosthetic vision. In Perceptual knowledge, ed. J. Dancy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  30. Macpherson, F. 2012. Cognitive penetration of colour experience: Rethinking the issue in light of an indirect mechanism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research 84: 24–62.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  31. Mishkin, M. 1982. A memory system in the monkey. Philosophical Royal Society of London Biology 298: 85–92.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  32. Nunn, J.A., L.J. Gregory, M. Brammer, S.C. Williams, D.M. Parslow, M.J. Morgan, R.G. Morris, E.T. Bullmore, S. Baron-Cohen, and J.A. Gray. 2002. Functional magnetic resonance imaging of synesthesia: Activation of V4/V8 by spoken words. Nature Neuroscience 5: 371–375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Piazza, M., A. Mechelli, C.J. Price, and B. Butterworth. 2006. Exact and approximate judgements of visual and auditorynumerosity: An fMRI study. Brain Research 1106: 177–188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  34. Ramachandran, V.S., and E.M. Hubbard. 2001. Synaesthesia: A window into perception, thought and language. Journal of Consciousness Studies 8: 3–34.Google Scholar
  35. Ramachandran, V.S., and E.M. Hubbard. 2003. The phenomenology of synaesthesia. Journal of Consciousness Studies 10: 49–57.Google Scholar
  36. Rissman, J., and A.D. Wagner. 2012. Distributed representations in memory: Insights from functional brain imaging. Annual Review of Psychology 63: 101–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  37. Serences, J.T., E.F. Ester, E.K. Vogel, and E. Awh. 2009. Stimulus-specific delay activity in human primary visual cortex. Psychological Science 20: 207–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Siegel, S. 2005. Which properties are represented in perception? In Perceptual experience, ed. T. Szabo Gendler and J. Hawthorne, 481–503. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  39. Siegel, S. 2006. How does visual phenomenology constrain object-seeing. Australasian Journal of Philosophy 84: 429–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Siegel, S. 2011. Cognitive penetrability and perceptual justification. Nous 46(2): 201–222. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-0068.2010.00786.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  41. Smilek, D., M.J. Dixon, C. Cudahy, and P.M. Merikle. 2001. Synaesthetic photisms influence visual perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 13: 930–936.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  42. Smilek, D., M.J. Dixon, and P.M. Merikle. 2003. Synaesthetic photisms guide attention. Brain and Cognition 53: 364–367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  43. Sperling, J.M., D. Prvulovic, D.E.J. Linden, W. Singer, and A. Stirn. 2006. Neuronal correlates of colour-graphemic synesthesia: A fMRI study. Cortex 42: 295–303.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  44. Squire, L.R. 1987. Memory and brain. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
  45. Squire, L.R., C.E. Stark, and R.E. Clark. 2004. The medial temporal lobe. Annual Review of Neuroscience 27: 279–306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  46. Treisman, A. 1982. Perceptual grouping and attention in visual search for features and for objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 8(2): 194–214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  47. Wagner, A.D., B.J. Shannon, I. Kahn, and R.L. Buckner. 2005. Parietal lobe contributions to episodic memory retrieval. Trends in Cognitive Science 9: 445–453.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Williamson, T. 2000. Knowledge and its limits. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Copyright information

© Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Departments of Philosophy, Center for Neurodynamics & Brogaard Lab for Multisensory ResearchUniversity of MissouriSt. LouisUSA

Personalised recommendations