Skip to main content

What Scientific Heroes Are (Not) Doing

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Imagination of Science in Education

Part of the book series: Cultural Studies of Science Education ((CSSE,volume 7))

  • 728 Accesses

Abstract

Graphs and graphing are quintessential images in and of science and scientific knowledge. However, as our research has shown, they constitute both an ideology—nature as understandable in terms of variables—and a representation of scientists as logical inquirers. However, when highly successful scientists are asked to interpret graphs in introductory textbooks of their own field, they often fail to provide the standard correct answers students in first-year university courses are expected to provide. That is, the science graphs and graphing practices that textbooks use to depict science processes and products do and require very different forms of knowledge than the ones that make good (successful) scientists. On the other hand, when they talk about graphs from their own or related work, the same scientists who fail to provide standard answers on textbook graphs exhibit the very knowledge and skills that lead to their successful research careers. Graphs in textbooks and the skills they require, therefore, contribute to the epic image of science rather than to science as practiced.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    “Best” applies both to the rhetorical force with which graphs can be employed in academic publications (e.g., Latour 1987) and to the effort of constituting a fact in the first place (e.g., Latour 1993).

  2. 2.

    Lacan (1966) deconstructs the system developed by Saussure using the tools of the very system for doing so. Much of the philosophical literature denoted by the adjective “deconstructionist” took up this task (e.g., Derrida 1972). It is a mirror view of the world that has also been critiqued in philosophy (e.g., Rorty 1979).

  3. 3.

    Derrida (1996) insists on the inherently social nature of all language: “We never speak but one language – and it exists asymmetrically, since it returns to the other, always for the other, from the other, kept by the other. Having come from the other, remaining with the other, returning to the other” (p. 70). It is therefore not surprising that scholars suggest reading to be a social process known through its achievement in the individual act of reading (Livingston 1995; Roth 2010a).

  4. 4.

    There are also articulated and nonarticulated nonconstituents (Barwise 1988), which we leave out because they are not central to the present discussion.

  5. 5.

    This issue was raised by some of the scientists who are used to situations where birthrates are calibrated in female, but death rates on entire (male + female) populations. As one theoretical ecologist pointed out, in the present case, it does not matter what the calibration is, for they all merely differ by a scaling factor.

  6. 6.

    Washroom (toilet) doors marked “Gentlemen” and “Women” or that have the symbols of men (with pants) and women (skirt) precisely constitute such a re-inscription of language into the world, where, in the Western world, these have real effects of dividing streams of living beings with urgent needs into two, precisely men and women.

  7. 7.

    Of course, these scientists did not have anything at stake in these graph-reading activities. Furthermore, to provide a situation where they could be as much at ease as possible without feeling like a “lab rat” (often mentioned early during our contacts with them), we refrained from pushing the respondents to provide at least some kind of answer.

  8. 8.

    Reference is considered here not as an indexical relationship that a given signifier bears to formations outside of semiotic systems but rather as a relation that has cross-modal implications (e.g., Preziosi 1986).

  9. 9.

    We use deconstruct in the way Derrida (e.g., 1981) proposed, not as destruction, but as a polysemic combination embodying Heidegger’s Abbau (taking apart an edifice) and Destruktion (dismantling).

  10. 10.

    A microanalysis of this scientist’s interpretative work was provided in (Bowen et al. 1999).

  11. 11.

    According to Eco (1984a), the “dynamic object,” the pulp of the material world, motivates signs. However, the material world cannot be rendered other than through the immediate object, that is, the content or interpretant of the sign. However, the form of the dynamic object is continuously mediated and changed through the formulations of immediate objects.

  12. 12.

    Or the activities of similar protagonists in The Name of the Rose (Eco 1984b) or Aramis or the Love of Technology (Latour 1992).

  13. 13.

    Because of this mediation, Eco (1984a) speaks of an “Immediate World,” which is always distinct from an inaccessible “Dynamic World” that provides the physical grounds of our existence, the world that we bodily inhabit.

  14. 14.

    We hold it with Wittgenstein (1953/1996), who suggests that the concept of “meaning” derives from a primitive understanding of the way language works. There is therefore no place in his pragmatic theory for this concept (Wittgenstein 2000).

  15. 15.

    We described such a process while studying the coevolution of signifying forms (scientific language, mathematical sign systems) and perception during high school students’ laboratory experiences (Roth 1996). As described above, it is during a phenomenological epoché that the entire process is inaugurated.

References

  • Bakhtine, M. [Volochinov, V. N.]. (1977). Le marxisme et la philosophie du langage: essai d’application de la mĂ©thode sociologique en linguistique. Paris: Les Éditions de Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barwise, J. (1988). On the circumstantial relation between meaning and content. In U. Eco, M. Santambrogio, & P. Violi (Eds.), Meaning and mental representations (pp. 23–39). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourdieu, P. (1980). Le sens pratique [Logic of practice]. Paris: Les Éditions de Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowen, G. M., Roth, W.-M., & McGinn, M. K. (1999). Interpretations of graphs by university biology students and practicing scientists: Towards a social practice view of scientific re-presentation practices. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 36, 1020–1043.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Saussure, F. (1916/1995). Cours de linguistique gĂ©nĂ©rale [Course on general linguistics]. Paris: Payot.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (1972). Marges de la philosophie [Marges of philosophy]. Paris: Les Éditions de Minuit.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (1981). Dissemination. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Derrida, J. (1996). Le monolinguisme de l’autre ou la prothèse d’origine [Monolingualism of the other or the prosthesis of origin]. Paris: GalilĂ©e.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eco, U. (1976). A theory of semiotics. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eco, U. (1984a). Semiotics and the philosophy of language. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eco, U. (1984b). The name of the rose. Boston: G. K. Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heidegger, M. (1977). Sein und Zeit [Being and time]. TĂĽbingen: Max Niemeyer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lacan, J. (1966). Écrits [Ecrits]. Paris: Seuil.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1987). Science in action: How to follow scientists and engineers through society. Milton Keynes: Open University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1992). Aramis ou l’amour des techniques [Aramis or the love of technology]. Paris: Éditions la DĂ©couverte.

    Google Scholar 

  • Latour, B. (1993). La clef de Berlin et autres leçons d’un amateur de sciences [The key to Berlin and other lessons of a science lover]. Paris: Éditions la DĂ©couverte.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lavoie, J. (1998, October 17). Moth imperils B.C. economy. Times Colonist, A1, A2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingston, E. (1995). An anthropology of reading. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lynch, M. (1991). Method: Measurement–ordinary and scientific measurement as ethnomethodological phenomena. In G. Button (Ed.), Ethnomethodology and the human sciences (pp. 77–108). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Preziosi, D. (1986). The multimodality of communicative events. In J. Deely, B. Williams, & F. E. Kruse (Eds.), Frontiers in semiotics (pp. 44–50). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rorty, R. (1979). Philosophy and the mirror of nature. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2001). “Authentic science”: Enculturation into the conceptual blind spots of a discipline. British Educational Research Journal, 27, 5–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2003a). Scientific literacy as an emergent feature of human practice. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 35, 9–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2003b). Toward an anthropology of graphing: Semiotic and activity theoretic perspectives. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2003c). Scientific literacy as an emergent feature of human practice. Journal of Curriculum Studies, 35, 9–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2009c). Radical uncertainty in scientific discovery work. Science, Technology & Human Values, 34, 313–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2010a). An anthropology of reading science texts in online media. Semiotica, 182, 409–442.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2010b). Vygotsky’s dynamic conception of the thinking-speaking relationship: A case study of science lectures. Pedagogies: An International Journal, 5, 49–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M. (2010c). Activism: A category for theorizing learning. Canadian Journal of Science, Mathematics, and Technology Education, 10, 278–291.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Bowen, G. M. (1999a). Complexities of graphical representations during lectures: A phenomenological approach. Learning and Instruction, 9, 235–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Bowen, G. M. (1999b). Digitizing lizards or the topology of vision in ecological fieldwork. Social Studies of Science, 29, 719–764.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth, W.-M., & Bowen, G. M. (2003). When are graphs ten thousand words worth? An expert/expert study. Cognition and Instruction, 21, 429–473.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharrock, W., & Button, G. (1991). The social actor: Social action in real time. In G. Button (Ed.), Ethnomethodology and the human sciences (pp. 137–175). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (1953/1996). Philosophische Untersuchungen/Philosophical investigations (2nd ed.). New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wittgenstein, L. (2000). Wittgenstein source: Bergen text edition. htpp://www.wittgensteinsource.org. Accessed 22 Sept 2010.

  • Roth, W.-M. (1996). The co-evolution of situated language and physics knowing. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 5, 171–191

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

van Eijck, M., Roth, WM. (2013). What Scientific Heroes Are (Not) Doing. In: Imagination of Science in Education. Cultural Studies of Science Education, vol 7. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-5392-1_2

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics