Skip to main content

The Interaction of Case with Aspect and Number

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Book cover Semantics of Genitive Objects in Russian

Part of the book series: Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory ((SNLT,volume 89))

  • 440 Accesses

Abstract

Chapter 7investigates the ways in which the choice of case is affected by aspect and number. Irrealis Genitive tends to be assigned to plural nominals, rather than to singular ones, and to complements of imperfective, rather than perfective, verbs. It will be shown that under negation, singular objects as well as objects of perfective verbs exhibit a tendency to carry existential commitment, a factor that rules out Irrealis Genitive. I argue that this tendency is derived from the semantically marked status of perfective aspect and singular morphology, compared to the default imperfective and plural forms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 99.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 129.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    A detailed discussion of this use of the imperfective can be found in Forsyth (1970), Comrie (1979), Smith (1991), Padučeva (1995) and Grønn (2003), among others.

  2. 2.

    This factor is contributed by perfective aspect, normally associated with telicity and, thus, with an event reaching its natural endpoint (e.g. Krifka 1992; Filip 1999, 2000, 2008). In the case of reading an article, the natural endpoint comes when the whole article is finished. Since in the discussed example, the object is plural, Dima is expected to finish reading a number of contextually presupposed articles.

  3. 3.

    It should be noted that (17a) does not require any of the scenarios mentioned above. Thus, it is acceptable even if the speaker has not expected Dima to find any documents and does not know of any search conducted by Dima. The sentence can be merely used to assert that it is not true that Dima has, by accident, come across any documents. Such an interpretation is not available for (17b). This sentence seems to presuppose either that Dima has been engaged in a search, or that the speaker has expected Dima to find some documents – whether by chance or as a result of looking for them.

  4. 4.

    An exception might be constituted by verbs of creation, in such sentences as He didn’t write a letter. However, even in these sentences, once it is assumed that an accomplishment event did take place but was not completed, one tends to conclude that the created object did come into existence, even though the creation process was not finished. Thus, if we hear that John didn’t finish writing a letter, we may conclude that a letter does exist, even though it is not completed. This, in turn, would mean that the object has a referent in the actual world, and is likely to be interpreted as an entity in that world, rather than a property. This approach is controversial, however. Thus, Parsons (1990) treats such NPs as a letterin the sentence above as denoting objects. In contrast, Landman (1992) does not accept this view.

  5. 5.

    Perfective aspect is compatible here with genitive case-marking. Perfectivity does not force a specific or presupposed reading of the nominal. The knowledge that the speaker was present in the room in question is sufficient to trigger an expectation that a seeing event would take place, i.e. that the speaker would see those objects that were present in the room, within her field of perception. Apparently, the presence of an expectation of this kind is sufficient to license perfective aspect. The object therefore need not receive a presupposed existence reading. In addition, existence of flowers in the room could be expected by some other discourse participant but not by the speaker. The speaker could then use the genitive form in order to emphasize that an expectation or commitment of this kind is groundless.

References

  • Bogusławski, A. 1985. The problem of the negated imperative in perfective verbs revisited. Russian Linguistics9: 225–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Borik, Olga. 2002. Aspect and reference time. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Comrie, Bernard. 1979. Definite and animate direct objects: A natural class. Linguistica Silesiana3: 13–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dahl, Osten. 1975. On generics. In Formal semantics of natural language, ed. E. Keenan, 99–111. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Dickey, Stephen M. 2000. Parameters of Slavic aspect: A cognitive approach. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diesing, Molly. 1992. Indefinites. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Farkas, Donka F., and Henriëtte de Swart. 2003. The semantics of incorporation. Stanford: CSLI.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filip, Hana. 1999. Aspect, eventuality types and nominal refrence. New York/London: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filip, Hana. 2000. The quantization puzzle. In Events as grammatical objects, ed. C. Tenny and J. Pustejovsky. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filip, Hana. 2003. Prefixes and the delimitation of events. Journal of Slavic Linguistics11(1): 55–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filip, Hana. 2008. Events and maximalization. In Theoretical and crosslinguistic approaches to the semantics of aspect, ed. Susan Rothstein. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Filip, Hana, and Susan Rothstein. 2006. Telicity as a semantic parameter. In Annual workshop on formal approaches to Slavic linguistics: The Princeton University meeting, ed. James Lavine, Steven Franks, Hana Filip and Mila Tasseva-Kurktchieva. Ann Arbor: Michigan Slavic Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Forsyth, James. 1970. A grammar of aspect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grønn, Atle. 2003. The semantics and pragmatics of Russian factual imperfective. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Oslo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jakobson, Roman O. 1957/1971. Shifters, verbal categories, and the Russian verb. In Selected writings II, ed. Roman O. Jakobson, 130–147. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, Olga. 2008. On the semantics of aspect and number. In Annual workshop on formal approaches to Slavic linguistics (FASL):The Stony Brook meeting, ed. Andrei Antonenko et al., 185–198. Ann Arbor: Michigan Slavic Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kagan, Olga. 2010. Russian aspect as number in the verbal domain. In Layers of aspect, ed. Brenda Laca and Patricia Hofherr, 125–146. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krifka, Manfred. 1992. Thematic relations as links between nominal reference and temporal constitution. In Lexical matters, ed. I. Sag and A. Szabolcsi, 29–53. Stanford: CSLI Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landman, Fred. 1992. The progressive. Natural Language Semantics1: 1–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lasersohn, Peter. 1990. A semantics for groups and events. New York/London: Garland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leinonen, M. 1982. Russian aspect, “Temporal’naja Lokalizacija” and definiteness / indefiniteness. Helsinki: Neuvostoliittionsttituutin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levinson, Dmitry. 2005. Aspect in negative imperatives and genitive of negation: A unified analysis of two phenomena in Slavic languages. Ms.

    Google Scholar 

  • Padučeva, Elena V. 1995. Vidovaja parnost' i količestvennyj predel dejstvija. Rusistika Segodnja3: 38–50.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parsons, T. 1990. Events in the semantics of English: A study in subatomic semantics. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pereltsvaig, Asya. 1998. Genitive of negation in Russian. Proceedings of IATL13: 167–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pereltsvaig, Asya. 1999. The genitive of negation and aspect in Russian. In McGill working papers in linguistics14, ed. Y. Rose and J. Steele, 111–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothstein, Susan. 2004. Structuring events. Oxford: Blackwell Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Sauerland, Uli. 2003. A new semantics for number. In Proceedings of SALT 13, ed. R. Young and Y. Zhou. Ithaca: CLC Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. 1991. The parameter of aspect. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelin, Nils B. 1990a. Verbal aspect in discourse: On the state of art. In Verbal aspect in discourse, ed. Nils B. Thelin. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelin Amsterdam, Nils B. 1990b. On the concept of time: Prolegomena to a theory of aspect and tense in narrative discourse. In Verbal aspect in discourse, ed. Nils B. Thelin. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Timberlake, Alan. 1986. Hierarchies in the genitive of negation. In Case in Slavic, ed. R.D. Brecht and J.S. Levine. Columbus: Slavica Publishers, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Kagan, O. (2013). The Interaction of Case with Aspect and Number. In: Semantics of Genitive Objects in Russian. Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, vol 89. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-5225-2_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics