Skip to main content

Constraints, Allomorphy and Form of Compound Constituents

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Compounding in Modern Greek

Part of the book series: Studies in Morphology ((SUMO,volume 2))

  • 562 Accesses

Abstract

The form of compounds and their constituents are examined in Chap. 7. It is argued that constraints, allomorphy and lexically specified features have a central role in the development of compound structures. The so-called bare-stem constraint is responsible for hiding the overt realisation of compound-internal derivational suffixes, while another one, the Optimal Syllable Constraint, affects the combination of the two compound members by selecting a specific allomorph as the right candidate to occupy the left-hand position. It is also argued that, most of the times, an item participates in a compound with a specific allomorph, which may arise phonologically or be lexically specified. Finally, it is shown that the form of a compound can be affected by the presence of learned items, originating from Ancient Greek, which may occupy both structural positions. Compounds combining learned elements are mainly found in scientific term formation.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    ksaná is a medieval formation, combining two AG preverbs, ek and aná. Initially, the result of this combination was a bound prefix, eksana-, which became a free adverb in MG (see Mendes-Dosuna 1997 for details). For the properties of formations with ksaná, the reader is referred to Rivero (1992), Smirniotopoulos and Joseph (1998) and Ralli (2004).

  2. 2.

    -vol(ó) is a bound stem. Stems of this type are examined in Chap. 10.

  3. 3.

    In this chapter, I will limit the discussion on the absence of derivational suffixes and will avoid considerations regarding the presence of absence of prefixes. It is generally known that the derivational status of several prefixes is not a clear-cut case (Chap. 11) and that in many respects several prefixes behave like the left-hand constituents of compounds. Suffice to mention the characteristics of stress subordination and categorial neutrality that are shared by the so called class II prefixes in English (e.g. pro- and en-, as in the words proclitic and enclitic) and the left-hand constituents of compounds (see, among others, Å tekauer 2005). Moreover, as noticed in Sect. 6.3.3, prefixes, like stems, participate in exocentric structures.

  4. 4.

    The origin of each example is listed in parenthesis.

  5. 5.

    As already noticed (Chap. 6, footnote 24), in the Northern Greek dialects, such as those of the islands of Lesbos and Imbros, there is high-vowel deletion and mid-vowel raising in unstressed position. Thus, the unstressed /o/ becomes /u/ and the unstressed /e/ becomes /i/ in the compounds kuklustsipázumi (< *kuklostsepázome) and maγirukinónu (< *maγirokenóno).

  6. 6.

    Ralli and Xydopoulos (to appear) have demonstrated that blends obey some rules of compound formation, but they are also different, since they are deliberate formations displaying a gradable structural and semantic opacity, depending on the speakers’ intentions to transmit a clear or opaque message. See Arvaniti (1998), Koutita and Fliatouras (2001) and Ralli and Xydopoulos (to appear) for detailed information regarding Greek blends.

  7. 7.

    The recent Greek dictionaries by Babiniotis (2002) and the Institute of Modern Greek Studies (1998) provide examples of the use of these terms.

  8. 8.

    /γ/ becomes the unvoiced /x/ in front of the also unvoiced /t/ (e.g. aniγ + t(ós) - > anixtós ‘open’).

  9. 9.

    The term ‘basic stem’ has been employed by Aronoff (1994) and Pirelli and Battista (2000) in order to denote a stem form that is synchronically unpredictable on the basis of another stem of the same paradigm.

  10. 10.

    This rule, however, does not apply to items of AG origin, as the following example illustrates:

    (i)

    éfstoxos / *épstoxos < ef-  stóxos

     

    lit. well-targeted   well  target

     

    ‘accurate’

    Since its application is not exceptionless, it could be characterised as a lexical phonological rule, along the lines of a lexical-phonology approach (Kiparsky 1982).

  11. 11.

    Non-phonologically motivated allomorphy can be generally found in the inflection of fusional languages, such as Latin and Greek. For instance, in Aronoff’s (1994) work, attention is brought to allomorphy displayed by the Latin verbs of the first conjugation class, like amare ‘to love’, which have three stem variants, depending on the context:

    (ii)

    a.

    ama- : present stem which derives from the root am and the thematic vowel /a/

     

    b.

    amav-: perfect stem, as in amavi ‘I have loved’, and

     

    c.

    amat-: participial stem, as in amatus ‘loved’.

    A major difference between Aronoff’s view of stems and the one adopted in this book is that for Aronoff, a stem is not a meaningful unit, while I opt for the morphematic status of stems, as items carrying form and meaning.

  12. 12.

    The same conclusion is reached by Booij (1997b) for Dutch and Maiden (1992) for the Romance languages.

  13. 13.

    An alternative solution would be to suppose that there is a single form X and that -δ- is inserted by a kind of morphological rule, as proposed by Thomadaki (1994). Since there is a range of formatives that can be added to basic stem forms, and not only -δ- (see kréa(s) ‘meat’ ~ kréa-t-(os) ‘of the meat’), depending on the particular noun one deals with, this solution presupposes that the selection of each formative is carried out by a special diacritic marker, which should characterise the lexical entry of the stem, and that each time, insertion is accomplished by a distinct rule for the specific formative. I would not opt for this solution because it presupposes a more complex system than the simple postulation of allomorphs.

  14. 14.

    Note that, as opposed to suffixes, prefixes are not affected by non-phonologically conditioned allomorphy. However, they are subject to lexical phonology (phonologically conditionned allomorphy). For instance, the negative prefix a- (e.g. á-γnostos ‘unknown’ assumes the form an- when the item it combines with begins with a vowel (e.g. an-étimos ‘not-ready’)).

  15. 15.

    There are also cases, where the compound marker is absent and the consonant-final allomorph is combined with items beginning with a vowel, like émboros ‘trader’ (e.g. somatémboros lit. body trader ‘pimp, procurer’). As argued in Sect. 2.3.4, the compound marker -o- does not usually show up when the second member of a compound begins with a vowel stronger than /o/ on the sonority hierarchy, in this case, /e/.

  16. 16.

    Neuter nouns in -i have exactly the same inflectional paradigm as the other productively built neuter nouns in -o (e.g. vunó ‘mountain’ in iii), with the exception of the syncretic nominative, accusative and vocative cases in the singular number (Appendix 2).

    (iii) NOM/ACC/VOC.SG vun-ó  versus xartí-Ø

     GEN.SG      vun-ú    xarti-ú

     NOM/ACC/VOV.PL vun-á     xarti-á

     GEN.PL      vun-ón      xarti-ón

    Historically, the two types of nouns were not distinct till the early Middle Ages (Browning 1983), when both types shared an -on ending in the singular number, namely, in the nominative, accusative and vocative case. For instance, compare the medieval form ospítion ‘house’ with the MG one spíti.

  17. 17.

    /i/ is pronounced as a semi-vowel /j/ in front of a stressed vowel.

  18. 18.

    Tentatively, another solution would be to suggest that the word-final /i/ of the neuter nouns has been reanalysed as part of the inflectional ending (−i, -iu, -ia, ion). This solution has the advantage of allowing the stem to have the same consonant-final form (e.g. xart-) in inflection, as well as in derivation (e.g. xárt-in-os ‘(made of) paper) and compounding (e.g. xart-ó-kuto ‘paper box’). There are reasons why I do not opt for this solution: first, /i/ appears in all paradigmatic forms, something which suggests that it belongs to a constituent whose form does not vary according to the morphosyntactic context, that is, to the stem. Second, the adoption of a new set of endings (−i, -iu, -ia, ion) is not economical, since it multiplies the set of endings for the neuter nouns. Third, the assumption that the final /i/ belongs to the inflectional ending would not explain why compounds such as konservokúti ‘tin box’ (< konsérv(a) ‘can, tin’ + kutí ‘box’) and θalasopúli ‘sea bird’ (< θálas(a) ‘sea’ + pulí ‘bird’) exist (see also 18ab). As shown in Sect. 5.4.3, these forms display a different stress position from that of the second constituent (the neuter noun in -i); when taken in isolation, a property which could be interpreted if the entire form in -i is taken to be a stem (i.e. a Xi stem) and not a fully inflected word. See Appendix 2 for the inflection of neuter nouns in –i.

  19. 19.

    In the singular number, feminine nouns in -i (tíxi ‘chance’), -a (xará ‘joy’), and -u (alepú ‘fox’) have a stem coinciding with a word form, except in the genitive case (e.g. tíxi-s/xará-s/alepú-s). In contrast, they present an overt ending in all plural forms (e.g. tíx-es/xar-és/alepúδ-es). See Appendix 2 and Ralli (2000, 2005) for more details on Greek nominal inflection.

  20. 20.

    After the independence war in the nineteenth century, and the liberation from the Ottoman empire, the creation of the modern Greek state has undergone a long struggle (the ‘language question’) between the written and spoken language, termed respectively Katharevousa ‘purist’ and Dimotiki ‘demotic, popular’, as to which would become the official language. SMG (officially established in 1976) is based mainly on Dimotiki but has assumed a strong influence from Katharevousa, which resulted in the ‘cleaning out’ (hence the term ‘purist’) of the greatest amount of loanwords and the retention of some archaic inflectional patterns (see Appendix 1 for details).

  21. 21.

    /n/ was deleted before the /s/ of the nominative singular suffix (γéron + s - > γéros), when the word changed inflection class, around the early Middle Ages (Browning 1983).

References

  • Anastasiadi-Symeonidi, Anna. 1996. I neoelliniki sinthesi [Modern Greek compounding]. In Zitimata neoellinikis glossas: Didaktiki prosengisi [Themes of the Modern Greek language: A didactic approach], ed. Georgia Katsimali and Photis Kavoukopoulos, 97–120. Rethymno: University of Crete.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andriotis, Nikolaos. 1957. Ta parataktika rimatika sintheta stin elliniki glossa [Coordinative verbal compounds in the Greek language]. In Memory of Manolis Triantphyllidis, 43–61. Thessaloniki: Institute of Modern Greek Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andriotis, Nikolaos. 1992. Etymologiko leksiko tis Koinis Neoellinikis [Etymological dictionary of Modern Greek Koine], 3rd ed. Thessaloniki: Institute of Modern Greek Studies

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, Mark. 1976. Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aronoff, Mark. 1994. Morphology by itself. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arvaniti, Amalia. 1998. Endiksis prosodiakis domis se prosfata leksika migmata tis Ellinikis [Indications for prosodic structure in recent Greek lexical blends]. Studies in Greek linguistics 18: 68–82.

    Google Scholar 

  • Babiniotis, George. 2002. Leksiko tis Neas Ellinikis glossas [Dictionary of the Modern Greek language]. Athens: Centre of Lexicography.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, Geert. 1997a. Autonomous morphology and paradigmatic relations. In Yearbook of morphology 1996, ed. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 35–54. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, Geert. 1997b. Allomorphy and the autonomy of morphology. Folia Linguistica XXXI 1–2: 25–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Booij, Geert. 2005. The grammar of words. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Browning, Robert. 1983. Medieval and Modern Greek, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carstairs, Andrew. 1988. Some implications of phonologically conditioned allomorphy. In Yearbook of morphology 1988, ed. Geert Booij and Jaap van Marle, 67–94. Dordrecht: Foris.

    Google Scholar 

  • Debrunner, Albert. 1917. Griechische Wortbildungslehre. Heidelberg: Carl Winter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatzidakis, Georgios. 1905–1907. Meseonika ke Nea Ellinika [Medieval and Modern Greek]. Athens: Sakellariou.

    Google Scholar 

  • Κarasimos, Athanasios. 2010. Thematiki allomorfia sta Nea Ellinika: O periorismos tou monadikou allomorfou [Stem allomorphy in Modern Greek: The single allomorph constraint]. Paper read at the 2nd Patras Conference of Students in Linguistics PICGL, Patras, June 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karasimos, Athanasios. 2011. Ipologistiki epeksergasia tis alomorfias stin paragogi lekseon tis Ellinikis [A computational treatment of allomorphy in Greek derivation]. PhD diss., University of Patras.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karasimos, Athanasios, Dimitra Melissaropoulou, Angela Ralli, Dimitris Papazachariou, and Dimitris Asimakopoulos. 2008. GreeD: Cataloguing and encoding Modern Greek dialectal spoken corpora. Paper read at the Conference of Cataloguing and Decoding dialectal corpora CatCod, Orleans, December 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kiparsky, Paul. 1982. Lexical morphology and phonology. In Linguistics in the morning calm, selected papers from SICOL-1981, ed. The Linguistic Society of Korea, 4–91. Seoul: Hanshin Publishing Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koutita, Myrto, and Asimakis Fliatouras. 2001. Blends in Greek dialects: A morphosemantic analysis. In Proceedings of the 1st International Conference of Modern Greek dialects and Linguistic Theory, ed. Angela Ralli, Brian Joseph, and Mark Janse, 117–130. Patras: University of Patras.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leksiko tis Koinis Neoellininikis [Dictionary of Modern Greek Koine]. 1998. Thessaloniki: Institute of Modern Greek Studies.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieber, Rochelle. 1980. On the organization of the lexicon. PhD diss., MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lieber, Rochelle. 1982. Allomorphy. Linguistic Analysis 10: 27–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maiden, Martin. 1992. Irregularity as a determinant of morphological change. Journal of Linguistics 28: 285–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manolessou, Ioanna, and Symeon Tsolakidis. 2009. Greek coordinated compounds: Synchrony and diachrony. Patras Working Papers in Linguistics 1: 23–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mendez-Dosuna, Julian. 1997. Fusion, fission, and relevance in language change: De-univerbation in Greek verb morphology. Studies in Language 21(3): 577–612.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nespor, Marina. 1999. Fonologia [Phonology]. Translation and adaptation to Greek by Angela Ralli of the book Fonologia (Bologna: Il Mulino 1992). Athens: Patakis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petrounias, Evangelos. 1985. Ta leksika tis Neas Ellinikis. I etymologies tus ke i etymologies tou Leksikou tou Idrymatos Traintaphylidi [Dictionaries of Modern Greek. Their etymologies and the etymologies of the Triantaphyllidis Foundation Dictionary]. Studies of the Greek Language 1985: 407–416.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pirrelli, Vito, and Mario Battista. 2000. The paradigm dimension of stem allomorphy in Italian verb inflection. Rivista di Linguistica 12(2): 307–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 1988. Eléments de la linguistique du grec modern: la structure du verbe. PhD diss., Université de Montréal.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 2000. A feature-based analysis of Greek nominal inflection. Glossologia 11–12: 201–228.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 2004. Stem-based versus word-based morphological configurations: The case of Modern Greek preverbs. Lingue e Linguaggio 2(2): 241–275.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 2005. Morfologia [Morphology]. Athens: Patakis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 2007a. I sinthesi lekseon: diaglossiki morfologiki prosengisi [The composition of words: A cross-linguistic morphological approach]. Athens: Patakis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 2007b. On the role of allomorphy in inflectional morphology: Evidence from Greek dialectal variation. In Open problems in linguistics and lexicography, ed. Giandomenico Sica, 89–109. Milano: Polimetrica.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 2009. Modern Greek V V dvandva compounds: A linguistic innovation in the history of the Indo-European language. Word Structure 2(1): 48–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela. 2012. Morphology in language contact: Verbal loanblend formation in Asia Minor Greek (Aivaliot). In Morphologies in contact, Studia Typologica 12, ed. M. Thomas Stolz, H.Otsuka Vanhove, and A. Urdze, 185–212. Berlin: Akademie Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela, and Athanasios Karasimos. 2009a. The bare-stem constraint in Greek compound formation. Gengo Kenkyu 135: 29–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela, and Athanasios Karasimos. 2009b. The role of constraints in compound formation. Lingue e Linguaggio 2009(1): 53–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ralli, Angela, and George Xydopoulos. To appear. Blend formation in Modern Greek (with G. Xydopoulos). In Cross-disciplinary perspectives on lexical blending (Trends in Linguistics), ed. Vincent Renner, François Maniez and Pierre Arnaud. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rivero, Maria Luisa. 1992. Adverb incorporation and the syntax of adverbs in Modern Greek. Linguistics and Philosophy 15: 289–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smirniotopoulos, Jane, and Brian D. Joseph. 1998. Syntax versus the lexicon: Incorporation and compounding in Modern Greek. Journal of Linguistics 34: 447–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Å tekauer, Pavol. 2005. Compounding and affixation. Any difference? In Morphology and its demarcations, ed. Wolfgang U. Dressler, Dieter Kastovsky, Oskar E. Pfeiffer, and Franz Rainer, 151–159. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomadaki, Evangelia. 1994. Morfologika provlimata tis Neoellinikis: i klisi tu usiastiku [Morphological problems of Modern Greek: The inflection of the noun]. PhD diss., University of Athens.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thomason, Sarah G. 2001. Language contact. An introduction. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Angela Ralli .

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Ralli, A. (2013). Constraints, Allomorphy and Form of Compound Constituents. In: Compounding in Modern Greek. Studies in Morphology, vol 2. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-4960-3_7

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics