Skip to main content

The Tripod Effect: Co-evolution of Cooperation, Cognition and Communication

  • Chapter
  • First Online:

Part of the book series: Biosemiotics ((BSEM,volume 6))

Abstract

This article concerns the co-evolution of hominin cooperation, communication and cognition. Certain hominin ecologies seem to have relied on cognitive foresight. The capacity of planning for future needs, combined with more developed cooperative skills, opened up the cognitive niche of cooperation towards future goals. Such cooperation requires complex intersubjectivity (theory of mind). We analyze five domains of intersubjectivity: emotion, desire, attention, intention, and belief; and argue that cooperation towards future goals requires, among other things, joint intentions (we-intentions). We scrutinize the cognitive and communicative conditions for reciprocal altruism, found in some species; and indirect reciprocity, a form of cooperation typical in the hominin line.

Sharing intentions and beliefs about the future requires communication about what is not present in the current environment. Symbols are efficient tools for this kind of communication, and we argue that the benefits of cooperation for the future selected for the evolution of symbolic communication. In line with recent models describing how indirect reciprocity might develop into an evolutionarily stable strategy, we emphasize the need for yet more complex intersubjectivity and symbolic communication, including a minimal syntax.

Our argumentation triangulates hominin cognition, cooperation, and communication, showing how these interdependent factors mutually reinforce each other over the course of evolution. The new take in this article is the combined analyses of cooperation and cognitive mechanisms. Finally, our theses are linked to archaeological evidence.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution.

Buying options

Chapter
USD   29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD   169.00
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD   219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD   219.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Learn about institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    Our strategy is thus similar to that of Whiten (1999). However, we put more emphasis on analyzing the sub-components in the three domains.

  2. 2.

    Detached representations are called secondary representations in Suddendorf and Whiten (2001) and decoupled representations in Sterelny (2003).

  3. 3.

    Nevertheless, it is fairly safe to conclude that some sort of cuing takes place in the present situation: a cue in the physical or “mental” environment; if not, it would be the result of chance, just popping up in the brain.

  4. 4.

    Which species used these stone tools is not fully established. It might have been several hominin species: for example, A. gahri, H. habilis, and later perhaps H. ergaster.

  5. 5.

    We have not found any other researcher who grades what is involved in intersubjectivity in this way. Tomasello (1999, p. 179) distinguishes between three levels of a child’s development: seeing others as agents, seeing others as intentional agents, and seeing others as mental agents. His second and third levels correspond roughly to our capacities 4 and 5. Baron-Cohen (1994) proposes that “mind reading” has four components: an intentionality detector (ID), an eye-direction detector (EDD), a shared-attention mechanism (SAM), and a theory-of-mind mechanism (ToMM). Wellman and Liu (2004) write about a “scaling of theory-of-mind tasks” when they address the “sequence of understandings” in children’s developing intersubjectivity. They do a meta-analysis of studies of children’s understandings of different forms of intentions, emotions, desires, and knowledge. There is a considerable overlap with the capacities presented here. Stern (1985) and Brinck (2008a) distinguish between interaffectivity, interattentionality, and interintentionality without emphasizing the distinct roles of desires and beliefs.

  6. 6.

    It is characteristic of facial signals, such as the play-face expressions of chimpanzees and gorillas, that they carry emotional rather than referential meaning.

  7. 7.

    Understanding the desires of others does not require representing their beliefs: thinking “A wants X” does not require thinking “A believes that X is most desirable.”. Thus, desires need not be “value beliefs” as Flavell et al. (1990) maintain. Similarly, ascribing a desire to somebody does not entail an ascription of explicit intention: thinking “A wants X” does not require thinking “A intends to choose X.” As long as the desired object is present or a physical representation of it exists in the shared context, the agents’ behaviorally manifest attentional states reveal their intentions to act (Brinck, 2001, 2004).

  8. 8.

    This question relates to the studies concerning other-regarding preferences presented in Section 10.2.3.

  9. 9.

    A possible exception is revealed in a study by Warneken and Tomasello (2006). They let three human-raised juvenile chimpanzees watch while a human attempted, but failed, to achieve a succession of goals. Sometimes the chimpanzees helped the humans, mainly in situations where the human reached for objects but failed to grasp them. By contrast, Warneken and Tomasello (2006) showed that 18-month-old human infants did help an adult experimenter in a large majority of the cases involving various scenarios. See also Warneken, Hare, Melis, Hanus, and Tomasello (2007).

  10. 10.

    Van Schaik, and Kappeler (2006, p. 15) write: “the three basic conditions for reputation are individual recognition, variation in personality traits, and curiosity about the outcome of interactions involving third parties.” In our opinion, these criteria are too weak, since they do not include that reputation is communicated and therefore requires flexible, referential communicative skills.

  11. 11.

    Partly following Donald (1991), Zlatev, Persson, and Gärdenfors (2005) argue that, in addition to protolanguage and language with syntax, one can distinguish communication systems based on dyadic and triadic mimesis. They argue that untrained apes can communicate at least via dyadic mimesis: e.g., by gestures (see also Pika et al., 2003). However, the natural gestural communication of apes does not constitute a protolanguage because it is not conventionalized (and thus not symbolic) and has only limited compositionality. On the other hand, the communications of Kanzi, Koko, and other language-trained apes satisfy the criteria for protolanguage. Jackendoff (1999) likewise distinguishes several stages in the evolution of syntactic structures.

  12. 12.

    Partly conventionalized compositional icons may be sufficient for many situations, although fully developed symbols are more efficient.

  13. 13.

    Arp (2006) argues, albeit from a different perspective, that scene visualization is unique to the Homo line.

  14. 14.

    This kind of local optimality is one of the four conditions that Dessalles (2007, pp. 173-174) presents for protolanguage.

  15. 15.

    In this context, it should be noted that Nowak and Sigmund’s (2005) model considers only two kinds of interaction: “good” and “bad.” It goes without saying that, in real groups, communication about reputation builds on finer nuances.

  16. 16.

    We wish to thank Miriam Haidle, Steven Mithen, and April Nowell for helpful discussions on these matters.

  17. 17.

    Thieme (1999) writes: “equipped with outstanding technical talents for processing wood, they were already in this early period qualified to plan a future large game hunt with special weapons, to organize it, to coordinate it and to successfully accomplish it” (our translation).

  18. 18.

    In agreement with our argument, Kuhn and Stiner (2008, p. 48) conclude: “if … beads and other forms of body ornamentation represent a new way of communicating, then it follows humans must already have been using symbols to communicate when ornaments first appeared in Paleolithic sites.”

  19. 19.

    We wish to thank Ernst Fehr for this suggestion.

References

  • Ambrose, S. (1998). Late Pleistocene human population bottlenecks, volcanic winter, and differentiation of modern humans. Journal of Human Evolution, 34, 623–651.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Arp, R. (2006). The environment of out Hominin ancestors, tool-usage, and scenario visualization. Biology and Philosophy, 21, 95–117.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atance, C. M., & O’Neill, D. K. (2005). The emergence of episodic future thinking in humans. Learning and Motivation, 36, 126–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod, R., & Hamilton, W. D. (1981). The evolution of cooperation. Science, 214, 1390–1396.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barclay, P. (2005). Reputational benefits for altruistic punishment. Evolution and Human Behaviour, 27, 325–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 13(5), 513–552.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (1990). Language and species. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bickerton, D. (2002). Foraging versus social intelligence in the evolution of protolanguage. In A. Wray (Ed.), The transition to language (pp. 207–225). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bird, R. (1999). Cooperation and conflict: The behavioral ecology of the sexual division of labor. Evolutionary Anthropology, 8(2), 65–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bräuer, J., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2009). Are apes inequity averse? New data on the token-exchange paradigm. American Journal of Primatology, 71, 175–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I. (1997). The indexical ‘I’. The first person in thought and language. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I. (2001). Attention and the evolution of intentional communication. Pragmatics and Cognition, 9(2), 255–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I. (2004). Joint attention, triangulation and radical interpretation: A problem and its solution. Dialectica, 58(2), 179–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I. (2008a). The role of intersubjectivity for the development of intentional communication. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine. C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 115–140). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I. (2008b). From similarity to uniqueness: Method and theory in comparative psychology. In L. S. Röska-Hardy & E. M. Neumann-Held (Eds.), Learning from animals? Examining the nature of human uniqueness (pp. 155–170). London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I., & Gärdenfors, P. (1999). Representation and self-awareness in intentional agents. Synthese, 118, 89–104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brinck, I., & Gärdenfors, P. (2003). Co-operation and communication in apes and humans, Mind and Language, 18(5), 484–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brownell, C. A., & Carriger, M. S. (1990). Changes in cooperation and self-other differentiation during the second year. Child Development, 61, 1164–1174.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Buckner R. L., & Carroll, D. C. (2007). Self-projection and the brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11, 49–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bugnyar, T., Stöwe, M., & Heinrich, B. (2004). Ravens, Corvus corax follow gaze direction of humans around obstacles. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B, 271, 1331–1336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burkart, J. M., Fehr, E., Efferson, C., & van Schaik, C. P. (2007). Other-regarding preferences in a non-human primate: Common marmosets provision food altruistically. PNAS, 104, 19762–19766.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (1999). A nonverbal false belief task: The performance of children and great apes. Child Development, 70(2), 381–395.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter, M., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2002). A new false belief test for 36-month-olds. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 20, 393–420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership. Econometrica, 74, 1579–1601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Correia, S. P. C., Dickinson, A., & Clayton, N. S. (2007). Western scrub-jays anticipate future needs independently of their current motivational state. Current Biology, 17, 856–861.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological and referential understanding of action in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B 29, 447–458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, T. (1997). The symbolic species. London: Penguin Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • D’Entremont, B. (2000). A perceptual-attentional explanation of gaze following in 3- to 6-months-olds.” Developmental Science, 3, 302–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dessalles, J.-L. (2007). Why we talk. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Waal, F. B. M., & Brosnan, S. F. (2006). Simple and complex reciprocity in primates. In P. M. Kappeler & C. P. van Schaik (Eds.), Cooperation in primates and humans (pp. 85–105). Berlin: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the modern mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donald, M. (1999). Preconditions for the evolution of protolanguages. In M. C. Corballis, & S. E. G. Lea (Eds.), The descent of mind (pp. 355–65). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar, R. (1996). Grooming, gossip and the evolution of language. London: Faber and Faber.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunin-Kepliz, B., & Verbrugge, R. (2001). A tuning machine for cooperative problem solving. Fundamenta Informatica, 21, 1001–1025.

    Google Scholar 

  • Emery, N. J. (2000). The eyes have it: the neuroethology, function and evolution of social gaze. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 24, 581–604.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Johnson, M. H., &Csibra, G. (2004). Mechanisms of eye gaze perception during infancy. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(8), 1320–1326.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E. (2004). Don’t lose your reputation. Nature, 432, 449–450.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2002). Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature, 415, 137–140.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Flavell, E. R., Green, F. L., & Moses, J. L. (1990). Young children's understanding of fact beliefs versus value beliefs. Child Development, 61(4), 915–928.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Franco, F. (2005). Infant pointing. In N. Eilan, C. Hoerl, T. McCormack, & J. Roessler (Eds.), Joint attention, communication, and other minds (pp. 129–164). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Gamble, C., & Steele, J. (1999). Hominid ranging patterns and dietary strategies. In H. Ullrich (Ed.), Hominid Evolution: Lifestyles and Survival Strategies (pp. 396–409). Leipzig: Edition Archaea.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (1996). Cued and detached representations in animal cognition, Behavioural Processes, 36, 263–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2000). Conceptual spaces: The geometry of thought. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2003). How homo became sapiens: On the evolution of thinking. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2004). Cooperation and the evolution of symbolic communication. In K. Oller & U. Griebel, (Eds.), The evolution of communication systems (pp. 237–256). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P. (2008). The role of intersubjectivity in animal and human cooperation. Biological Theory, 3(1), 1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gärdenfors, P., & Osvath M (2010). The evolution of anticipatory cognition as a precursor to symbolic communication. In R. K. Larson, V. Déprez, & H. Yamokido (Eds.), The evolution of human language: Biolinguistic perspectives (pp. 103–114). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Garnham, W. A., & Ruffman, T. (2001). Doesn’t see, doesn’t know: Is anticipatory looking really related to understanding or belief. Developmental Science, 4, 94–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naive theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287–292.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert D. T., & Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the future. Science, 317, 1351–1354.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20(1), 1–55.

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, J. C. (2004). Apes, monkeys, children, and the growth of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gomez, J. C. (2007). Pointing behaviors in apes and human infants: A balanced interpretation. Child Development, 78(3), 729–734.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gopnik, A., & Astington, J. W. (1988). Children’s understanding of representational change, and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26–37.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gulz, A. (1991). The planning of action as a cognitive and biological phenomenon. Lund: Lund University Cognitive Studies 2.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haidle, M. (2009). How to think a simple spear? In S. de Beaune, F. Coolidge, & T. Wynn (Eds.), Cognitive archaeology and human evolution (pp. 57–75). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., & Tomasello, M. (2000). Chimpanzees know what conspecifics do and do not see, Animal Behaviour, 59, 771–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hare, B., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2001). Do chimpanzees know what conspecifics know? Animal Behaviour, 61, 139–151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hauser, M. D., Chen, M. K., Chen, F., & Chuang, E. (2003). Give unto others: Genetically unrelated cotton-top tamarin monkeys preferentially give food to those who altruistically give food back. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B 270, 2363–2370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawkes, K. J., O’Connell, F., Blurton, N. G., Alvarez, J. H., & Charnov E. L. (1998). Grandmothering, menopause, and the evolution of human life histories. PNAS, 95, 1336–1339.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jackendoff, R. (1999). Possible stages in the evolution of the language capacity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(7), 272–279.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, K., Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Chimpanzees are rational maximizers in an ultimatum game. Science, 318, 107–109.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, S., Zlatev, J., & Gärdenfors, P. (2006). Why don’t chimps talk and humans sing like canaries? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(3), 287–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kacelnik, A. (2003). The evolutioin of patience. In G. Lowenstein, D. Read, & R. F. Baumeister (Eds.), Time and decsioin: Economic and psychological perspectives on intertemporal choice (pp. 115–138). New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kaminski, J., Riedel, J. Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2005). Domestic goats, Capra hircus, follow gaze direction and use social cues in an object task. Animal Behaviour, 69, 11–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1921). Zur psychologie des schimpansen. Psychologische Forschung, 1, 2–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, S. L., & Stiner, M. C. (2008). Paleolithic ornaments: Implications for cognition, demography and identity. Diogenes, 214, 40–48.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Bard, K. A. (2005). Understanding the point of chimpanzee pointing: Epigenesis and ecological validity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 185–189.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leavens, D. A., Hopkins, W. D., & Bard, K. A. (2008). The heterochronic origins of explicit reference. In J. Zlatev, T. Racine, C. Sinha, & E. Itkonen (Eds.), The shared mind: Perspectives on intersubjectivity (pp. 187–214). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, L., & Keller, L. (2006). The evolution of cooperation and altruism – A general framework and a classification of models. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 19, 1365–1376.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Leimar, O., & Hammerstein, P. (2001). Evolution of cooperation through indirect reciprocation. Proceedings of the Royal Society London, B 268, 745–753.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leonard W., & Robertson, M. L. (2000). Ecological correlates for home range variation in primates: Implications for human evolution. In S. Boinski & P. A. Garber (Eds.), On the move: How and why animals travel in groups (pp. 628–648). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lewis, D. K. (1969). Convention. Boston: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lindgren, K. (1997). Evolutionary dynamics in game-theoretic models. In B. Arthur, S. Durlauf, & D. Lane (Eds.), The economy as an evolving complex system II (pp. 337–367). Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maynard Smith, J. (1982). Evolution and the theory of games. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mulcahy, N. J., & Call, J. (2006). Apes save tools for future use. Science, 312, 1038–1040.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Naqshbandi, M., & Roberts W. A. (2006). Anticipation of future events in squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) and rats (Rattus norvegicus): Test of the Bischof-Kohler Hypothesis. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 120, 345–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, W., & Davidson, I. (1996). Human evolution, language and mind: A psychological and archaeological inquiry. Melbourne: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak, M. A., & Sigmund, K. (2005). Evolution of indirect reciprocity. Nature, 437, 1291–1298.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connell, J. F., Hawkes, K., & Blurton Jones, N. G. (1999). Grandmothering and the evolution of Homo erectus. Journal of Human Evolution, 36, 461–485.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osvath, M. (2009). Spontaneous planning for future stone throwing by a male chimpanzee. Current Biology, 19, 190–191

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Osvath, M., & Gärdenfors, P. (2005). Oldowan culture and the evolution of prospective cognition. Lund: Lund University Cognitive Studies, 122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osvath, M., & Osvath, H. (2008). Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and orangutan (Pongo abelii) forethought: self-control and pre-experience in the face of future tool use. Animal Cognition, 11, 661–674.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Panchanathan, K., & Boyd, R. (2004). Indirect reciprocity can stabilize cooperation without the second-order free rider problem. Nature, 432, 499–502.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Perner, J., Leekam, S., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-old’s difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 125–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pika, S., Liebal, K., & Tomasello, M. (2003). Gestural communication in young gorillas (Gorilla gorilla): Gestural repertoire and use. American Journal of Primatology, 60(3), 95–111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plummer, T. (2004). Flaked stones and old bones: Biological and cultural evolution at the dawn of technology. Yearbook of Physical Anthropology, 47, 118–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Povinelli, D. J., & Eddy, T. J. (1996). What young chimpanzees know about seeing. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 61(2), Serial No. 247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 4, 515–526.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Preston, S. D., & de Waal, F. (2003). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximal bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raby, C. R., Alexis, D. M., Dickinson, A., & Clayton, N. S. (2007). Planning for the future by western scrub-jays. Nature, 445, 919–921.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2003). On being the object of attention: Implications for self-other consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7(9), 397–402.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Repacholi, B., & Gopnik, A. (1997). Early understanding of desires: Evidence from 14 and 18 month olds. Developmental Psychology, 33, 12–21.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R. T., & Heinrich, J. (2003). Cultural evolution of human cooperation. In P. Hammerstein (Ed.), The Genetic and Cultural Origins of Cooperation (pp. 357–388). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, W. A. (2002). Are animals stuck in time? Psychological Bulletin, 128, 473–489.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, W. A. (2006). The questions of temporal and spatial displacement in animal cognition. In E. A. Wasserman & T. R. Zentall (Eds.), Comparative cognition: Experimental explorations of animal intelligence (pp. 145–163). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rosati, A. G., Stevens, J. R., Hare, B., & Hauser, M. D. (2007). The evolutionary origins of human patience: Temporal preferences in chimpanzees, bonobos, and human adults. Current Biology, 17, 1663–1668.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Searle, J. (1995). The construction of social reality. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Semmann, D., Krambeck, H.-J., & Milinski, M. (2005). Reputation is valuable within and outside one’s own social group. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 57, 611–616.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shennan, S. (2001). Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 11(1), 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sigmund, K., Hauert, C., & Nowak, M. (2001). Reward and punishment. PNAS, 98, 10757–10762.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Silk, J. B., Brosnan, S. F., Vonk, J., Henrich, J., Povinelli, D. J., Richardson, S. J., et al. (2005). Chimpanzees are indifferent to the welfare of unrelated group members. Nature, 437, 1357–1359.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Sterelny, K. (2003). Thought in a hostile world. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, W., McLinn, C. M., & Stevens, J. R. (2002). Discounting and reciprocity in an iterated prisoner’s dilemma. Science, 298, 2216–2218.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stevens, J. R., & Hauser, M. (2004). Why be nice? Psychological constraints on the evolution of cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 60–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Striano, T., & Bertin, E. (2005). Social-cognitive skills between 5 and 10 months of age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23, 1–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Busby, J. (2005). Making decisions with the future in mind. Learning and Motivation, 36, 110–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental time travel and the evolution of human mind. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133–167.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Corballis, M. C. (2007). The evolution of foresight: What is mental time travel and is it unique to humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299–351.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Suddendorf, T., & Whiten, A. (2001). Mental evolution an development: Evidence for secondary representation in children, great apes, and other animals. Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 629–650.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thieme, H. (1997). Lower Paleolithic hunting spears from Germany. Nature, 385, 807–810.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Thieme, H. (1999) Altpaläolithische Holzgeräte aus Schöningen, Lkr. Helmstadt. Germania, 77, 451–487.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., & Call, J. (2006). Do chimpanzees know what others see – or only what they are looking at? In S. Hurley & M. Nudds (Eds.), Rational animals (pp. 371–384). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Toth, N. (1985). The Oldowan reassessed: A close look at early stone artifacts. Journal of Archeological Science, 12, 101–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C., & Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: Confidence, confiding, and acts of meaning in the first year. In A. Lock (Ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 183–229). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 35–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381–403). New York: New York Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tulving, E. (2005). Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In H. Terrace & J. Metcalfe (Eds.), The missing link in cognition: Evolution of self-knowing consciousness (pp. 3–56). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Schaik. C. P., & Kappeler, P. M. (2006). Cooperation in primates and humans: closing the gap. In C. P. van Schaik & P. M. Kappeler (Eds.), Cooperation in primates and humans (pp. 85–105). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villa, P. (1983). Terra amata and the middle pleistocene archaeological record of Southern France. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, F., Hare, B., Melis, A. P., Hanus, D., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Spontaneous altruism by chimpanzees and young children. PloS Biology, 5, 1414–1420.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Wellman, H. M., & Liu, D (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development, 75, 523–541.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whiten, A. (1999). The evolution of deep social mind in humans. In M. C. Corballis & S. E. G. Lea (Eds.), The descent of mind: Psychological perspectives on hominid evolution (pp. 173–193). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson, G. S. (1984). Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature, 308, 181–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection – A selection for a handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 53, 205–214.

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Zlatev, J., Persson, T., & Gärdenfors, P. (2005). Bodily mimesis as the “missing link” in human cognitive evolution. Lund: Lund University Cognitive Studies, 121.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Most of the work for this article was done as part of the EU project Stages in the Evolution and Development of Sign Use (SEDSU). We want to thank the participants of the project for helpful comments and ideas. Peter Gärdenfors gratefully acknowledges support as a Senior Individual Researcher from the Swedish Research Council as well as support from the Linnaeus project Thinking in Time: Cognition, Communication and Learning during the final stages of the work. The research of Ingar Brinck was financed by the Bank of Sweden Tercentenary Foundation through the Centre for Cognitive Semiotics at Lund University.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Peter Gärdenfors .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2012 Springer Science+Business Media B.V.

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Gärdenfors, P., Brinck, I., Osvath, M. (2012). The Tripod Effect: Co-evolution of Cooperation, Cognition and Communication. In: Schilhab, T., Stjernfelt, F., Deacon, T. (eds) The Symbolic Species Evolved. Biosemiotics, vol 6. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2336-8_10

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics