Skip to main content

Sensuous Cognition

  • Chapter
  • First Online:
Visual Mathematics and Cyberlearning

Part of the book series: Mathematics Education in the Digital Era ((MEDE,volume 1))

Abstract

In the first part of this chapter I sketch an approach where human cognition is conceptualized in non-dualistic, non-representational, and non-computational terms. The basic idea is that cognition is a feature of living material bodies characterized by a capacity for responsive sensation. The sketched approach, which I term sensuous cognition, refers precisely to this view where sensation is considered to be the substrate of mind, and all psychic activity (cognitive, affective, volitional, etc.). I argue that, as far as humans are concerned, responsive sensation evolves—both at the phylogenetic and ontogenetic levels—interwoven with the material culture in which individuals live and growth. As a result, human cognition can only be understood as a culturally and historically constituted sentient form of creatively responding, acting, feeling, transforming, and making sense of the world. In the second part of the chapter, I present classroom experimental data involving 7–8-year-old students dealing with pattern recognition. The classroom data allow me to illustrate the interplay of the various sensuous modalities in mathematical cognition. I end the chapter suggesting that a sensuous-based materialistic monistic view of cognition needs to attend not only to the plethora of sensorial modalities that teachers and students display while engaging in mathematical activities, but also to the manner in which sensorial modalities come to constitute more and more complex psychic wholes of sensorial and artifactual units.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Chapter
USD 29.95
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
eBook
USD 84.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
Softcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
Hardcover Book
USD 109.99
Price excludes VAT (USA)
  • Durable hardcover edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Notes

  1. 1.

    For some embodied perspectives in mathematics education, see the special isssue of Educational Studies in Mathematics edited by Edwards, Radford, and Arzarello (2009), and the special issue edited by Radford, Schubring, and Seeger (2011) in the same journal. See also Bautista and Roth (2011) and the seminal book of Lakoff and Núñez (2000).

  2. 2.

    My use of the adjective objective does not refer to claims about truth. It is rather a claim about something that is distinct from the organism, something that objects the organism; in other words, the term objective refers to something that we can term Otherness.

References

  • Bautista, A., & Roth, W.-M. (2011). Conceptualizing sound as a form of incarnate mathematical consciousness. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 79, 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bateson, G. (1973). Steps to an ecology of mind. Frogmore, UK: Paladin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Edwards, L., Radford, L., & Arzarello, F. (Eds.). (2009). Gestures and multimodality in the teaching and learning of mathematics. Special Issue of Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 91–215

    Google Scholar 

  • Gee, J. P. (2003). What video games have to teach us about learning and literacy. New York: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gehlen, A. (1988). Man. His nature and place in the world. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, J. C. (2004). Apes, monkeys, children, and the growth of mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gogtay, N., Giedd, J., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(21), 8174–8179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hegedus, S., & Moreno-Armella, L. (2011). The emergence of mathematical structures. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 77(2–3), 369–388.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey, G. (1962). Introduction. In J.-M.-G. Itard (Ed.), The wild boy of Aveyron (pp. v–xxiv). New York: Meredith Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hyppolite, J. (1961). Logique et existence [logic and eistence]. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.

    Google Scholar 

  • Itard, J.-M.-G. (1962). The wild boy of Aveyron. New York: Meredith Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kant, I. (1787). Critique of pure reason. New York: St. Marin’s Press (Second Printing, 1965).

    Google Scholar 

  • Köhler, W. (1951). The mentality of apes. New York/London: The Humanities Press/Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lakoff, G., & Núñez, R. (2000). Where mathematics comes from. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Le Breton, D. (2009). El sabor del mundo. Una antropología de los sentidos [The taste of the world. An anthropology of the senses]. Buenos Aires, Argentina: Ediciones Nueva Visión.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leont’ev, A. N. (1978). Activity, consciousness, and personality. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leontyev [or Leont’ev], A. N. (2009). Activity and consciousness. Pacifica, CA: MIA. Retrieved August 29, 2009, from http://www.marxists.org/archive/leontev/works/activity-consciousness.pdf

  • Lewkowicz, D., & Lickliter, R. (Eds.). (1994). The development of intersensory perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lickliter, R., & Bahrick, L. E. (2000). The development of infant intersensory perception: Advantages of a comparative convergent-operations approach. Psychological Bulletin, 126(2), 260–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R. (1984). Sensación y percepción [Sensation and perception]. Barcelona, Spain: Ediciones Martínez Roca.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luria, A. R., & Vygotsky, L. S. (1998). Ape primitive man and child. Essays in the history of behavior. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press LLC.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malafouris, L., & Renfrew, C. (Eds.). (2010). The cognitive life of things: Archaeology, material engagement and the extended mind. In The cognitive life of things: Recasting the boundaries of the mind (pp. 1–12). Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marks, L. U. (2000). The skin of the film: Intercultural cinema, embodiment, and the senses. Durham, UK/London: Duke University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maturana, H., & Varela, F. (1998). The tree of knowledge. Boston: Shambhala. (Original work published 1987).

    Google Scholar 

  • McLuhan, M. (1962). The Gutenberg galaxy: The making of typographic man. Toronto, ON, Canada: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peirce, C. S. (1931). Collected papers (Vol. I-VIII). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2002). The seen, the spoken and the written. A semiotic approach to the problem of objectification of mathematical knowledge. For the Learning of Mathematics, 22(2), 14–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2008). Iconicity and contraction: A semiotic investigation of forms of algebraic generalizations of patterns in different contexts. ZDM – The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 40(1), 83–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2009). “No! He starts walking backwards!”: Interpreting motion graphs and the question of space, place and distance. ZDM – The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 41, 467–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2010a). Algebraic thinking from a cultural semiotic perspective. Research in Mathematics Education, 12(1), 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2010b). The eye as a theoretician: Seeing structures in generalizing activities. For the Learning of Mathematics, 30(2), 2–7.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2011a). Embodiment, perception and symbols in the development of early algebraic thinking. In Proceedings of the 35th conference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (pp. 17–24). Ankara, Turkey: PME.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2011b). Grade 2 students’ non-symbolic algebraic thinking. In J. Cai & E. Knuth (Eds.), Early algebraization (pp. 303–322). Berlin, Germany: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2011c, November 2–3). Phenomenological mediations: Changing the subject (and the object!). Working Paper presented at the Phenomenology and Mathematics Education φ-ME First Meeting, Turin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L. (2012). On the development of early algebraic thinking. PNA, 6(4), 117–133.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., Bardini, C., & Sabena, C. (2007). Perceiving the general: The multisemiotic dimension of students’ algebraic activity. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38, 507–530.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., Demers, S., Guzmán, J., & Cerulli, M. (2003). Calculators, graphs, gestures, and the production meaning. In P. Pateman, B. Dougherty, & J. Zilliox (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27 conference of the international group for the psychology of mathematics education (Vol. 4, pp. 55-62). University of Hawaii, PME.

    Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., Edwards, L., & Arzarello, F. (2009). Beyond words. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 70(2), 91–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., & Roth, W.-M. (2011). Intercorporeality and ethical commitment: An activity perspective on classroom interaction. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 77(2–3), 227–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Radford, L., Schubring, G., & Seeger, F. (Eds.). (2011). Signifying and meaning-making in mathematics thinking, teaching and learning: Semiotic perspectives. Special Issue of Educational Studies in Mathematics, 77(2–3), 149–397.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sheets-Johnstone, M. (2009). The corporeal turn. Exeter, Devon, UK: imprint-academic.com.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seitz, J. A. (2000). The bodily basis of thought. New Ideas in Psychology, 18, 23–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stephens, M. (1998). The rise of the image the fall of the world. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trend, D. (2010). The end of reading. From Gutenberg to grand theft auto. New York: Peter Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F. (1999). Ethical know-how. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. (Original work published 1992).

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1987–1999). Collected works. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vygotsky, L. S., & Luria, A. (1994). Tool and symbol in child development. In R. V. D. Veer & J. Valsiner (Eds.), The Vygotsky reader (pp. 99–174). Oxford, UK: Blackwell Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, F. (1998). The hand. How its use shapes the brain, language, and human culture. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 625–636.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wessnitzer, J., & Webb, B. (2006). Multimodal sensory integration in insects—Towards insect brain control architectures. Bioinspiration & Biomimetics, 1(3), 63–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaporozhets, A. V. (2002). The development of sensations and perceptions in early and preschool childhood. Journal of Russian and East European Psychology, 40(2), 22–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This chapter is a result of a research program funded by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC/CRSH). A previous version was presented at the ICME-12 Topic Study Group 22 (Learning and cognition in mathematics, co-chaired by Hsin-Mei Huang and Gaye William), South Korea, July 2012.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Luis Radford .

Editor information

Editors and Affiliations

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2013 Springer Science+Business Media Dordrecht

About this chapter

Cite this chapter

Radford, L. (2013). Sensuous Cognition. In: Martinovic, D., Freiman, V., Karadag, Z. (eds) Visual Mathematics and Cyberlearning. Mathematics Education in the Digital Era, vol 1. Springer, Dordrecht. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-007-2321-4_6

Download citation

Publish with us

Policies and ethics